No items found.

View Article PDF

Dizziness is reported to be one of the most common reasons for patients consulting a general practitioner.1 Although not all consultations regarding dizziness are related to the vestibular system, for example, they can be due to cardiovascular dysfunction, approximately 20–50% are believed to be due to balance disorders related to the peripheral vestibular system (see Table 1).1 Recent studies have estimated that 35% of US adults over the age of 35 years suffer from vestibular disorders, increasing to 85% aged 80 and over.2 Unfortunately, no reliable statistics are available on the prevalence of vestibular disorders for New Zealand. However, the Health Quality and Safety Commission reported that even between January and March, 2020, there were 65,893 fall injuries reported to the ACC, of which 8,544 were classified as serious.3 Overseas, impairment of the vestibular system has been estimated to increase the odds of falling by over 12-fold, and nearly 30% of adults aged 65 and over fall each year.2,4,5 In 2016, the mortality rate associated with falls in the US was estimated to be 122.2 per 100,000 persons.4,5

Table 1: Common causes of dizziness in primary care practice. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

The impact of vestibular dysfunction on loss of balance and falls is due both to its effects on brainstem vestibular reflex pathways as well as higher cognitive processing of self-motion signals from the vestibular system. The vestibular system (Figure 1) senses head movement (strictly speaking ‘head acceleration’ or change in head velocity) in different planes, as well as linear acceleration by gravity.6 The three semi-circular canals in each inner ear sense angular rotation of the head, and the two otoliths, the utricle and the saccule, sense linear movement.7,8 This linear movement not only includes movement of the head, forward and backward, and left and right, but linear acceleration of the head by gravity; the saccule, in particular, senses linear acceleration by gravity (Figure 2 for explanation).7 The utricle and saccule detect linear acceleration as a result of ‘otoconia’ (calcium carbonate crystals) that generate an inertial force on the hair cells during head movement, causing a change in electrical potential (see Figure 2).7 In this respect, it is important to note that the most primitive form of the otoliths (‘statoliths’) are estimated to have evolved approximately 670 million years ago, and they exist in invertebrates such as jellyfish. This is their only means of detecting upright, which is necessary for survival. Therefore, given their evolutionary age, the otoliths might be expected to have developed major contributions to balance in humans.9,10 The vestibular system, through short-latency brainstem pathways, generates rapid eye movements that compensate for the unintentional movement of the head, eg, movement of the head due to the pulse beat (the vestibulo-ocular reflexes or VORs) and maintains the stability of the visual image of the world on the retina.6 The vestibular system also generates rapid vestibulo-spinal reflexes (VSRs) which adjust posture for unintentional movement, enabling us to keep our balance.6 Without a normal vestibular system, vision would become blurred (a condition known as ‘oscillopsia’) and balance and locomotion become disrupted.6

Figure 1: Human inner ear anatomy. The cochlea and vestibular system, which encompasses the three semicircular canals (horizontal, superior (anterior vertical) and posterior vertical) and the two otolith organs (utricle and saccule), are depicted. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Figure 2: (A) Schematic representation of the plates of the otolithic receptors (the utricular and saccular maculae). The arrows show the preferred polarization of the hair cell receptors across the maculae. The dashed lines are lines of polarity reversal (lpr). The striola refers to a band of receptors on either side of the lpr. Schematics of type I (B,D) and type II receptors (C,E) show how linear acceleration acts on the otoliths and so deflects the hair bundles of individual receptors. From Curthoys et al7 with permission.

Information about angular and linear head movement is also transmitted to higher centres of the brain, where it contributes to the conscious experience of moving through the environment and to cognitive processes such as memory.11–15 As we move through the environment, the vestibular hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths detect every head movement, and transmit this information to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, where it is assimilated and stored to provide a spatial map of our movements.11–15 This information is integrated with other sensory information, such as that from the visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory and proprioceptive systems, and formulated into mathematical maps of the spatial world, allowing us to navigate through it more effectively.11–15 In the 1970s, specific neurons were discovered in the hippocampus that selectively discharged in response to specific areas of the environment. These became known as ‘place cells’.16 In the 1990s, related cells were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex, known as ‘grid cells’, which discharged in response to multiple areas in the environment.17 In 2014, the Nobel Prize in Medicine or Physiology was awarded to John O’Keefe, Edvard Moser and Britt-Mayer Moser for these discoveries, which have become known as the brain’s ‘global positioning system’.18 Since then, both place cells and grid cells have been demonstrated to rely on vestibular information from the inner ear.11–13 Therefore, one reason why vestibular-related dizziness contributes to falls, is that not only does it impair fast vestibular reflexes such as the VORs and VSRs, but it impairs the ability of the brain to integrate self-motion information and to navigate through the spatial environment and form spatial memories. Information from the vestibular system is distributed widely throughout the central nervous system and is involved in higher cognitive function.11–15,18 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths may be important for cognitive processing independently of the semi-circular canals;19 this is one reason why the evolutionary age of the otoliths is of interest.

In recent years, a substantial amount of epidemiological evidence has been published to support the idea that age-related hearing loss is a risk factor for dementia.20 For example, in a seminal study published in the Lancet, it was reported that the contribution of hearing loss to the incidence of dementia was greater than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity and social isolation.20 This result seemed surprising, because sensory systems had never been considered particularly important to dementia, except perhaps for olfactory function as a potential biomarker.21 It is important to note that the Livingstone et al study20 was based on data from high-income countries and the evidence from low-to-middle income countries is less convincing in this respect.22 Over the last several years, further evidence in support of the importance of hearing loss for the development of dementia has been published, although it is not entirely consistent.22–27

Less attention has been given to the other part of the inner ear, the vestibular system (see Figure 1); however, evidence is mounting that age-related vestibular disorders could also be a significant risk factor for the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia, along with hearing loss.28 The vestibular system is known to degenerate with age, as with other sensory systems, with decreases in hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths, a reduction in the number of neurons in the vestibular nerve and brainstem vestibular nucleus, and a deterioration of vestibular reflex responses.1,2 As shown in the Iceberg model in Figure 3, clinical presentation of age-related vestibular symptoms usually includes ‘presbystasis’ (the imbalance of disequilibrium) or ‘presbyvertigo’ (vertigo), or both, possibly with a decrease in vestibular perception; however, it is possible for age-related vestibular symptoms to be sub-clinical and therefore harder to detect .

Figure 3: Iceberg model of age-related vestibular loss.

VSR: vestibulo-spinal reflex; VOR: vestibulo-ocular reflex. ‘Presbystasis’: age-related imbalance of disequilibrium. ‘Presbyvertigo’: age-related vertigo. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Animal studies supporting the role of the vestibular system in cognitive function

The literature relating the vestibular system to cognitive function, especially spatial memory, dates back to the 1960’s.14 Numerous studies have reported evidence that unilateral or bilateral lesions of the peripheral vestibular system impair spatial memory in various maze and foraging tasks.14,29–31 In some cases, these have been conducted even 14 months after bilateral vestibular lesions (BVL) in rats, and the spatial memory deficits remain.32 A variety of potentially confounding factors have been controlled for, including vision, degree of motor activity, anxiety and auditory function, and the results have been consistent.29–32 BVL has been demonstrated to impair the function of neurons in the hippocampus that encode places in the environment (‘hippocampal place cells’),11,12 EEG activity in the theta frequency range,33–35 which is thought to regulate place cell function, and theta EEG activity among grid cells of the entorhinal cortex.13 Neurons in the thalamus which encode head direction (‘head direction cells’), are also dysfunctional following BVL.36 Together, these abnormalities in the function of place cells, grid cells and head direction cells, are likely to underlie the spatial memory deficits observed in animals.14,15 Furthermore, transgenic mice without otoconia and therefore without otolith function (‘otolith deficient tilted mice’), but with normal semi-circular canal function, have been shown to have aberrant hippocampal place cell and head direction cell activity.36,37 In addition, a variety of neurochemical changes have been documented in the hippocampus following BVL, including changes in the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype of glutamate receptor and muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptors,38–40 both of which are implicated in hippocampal learning and memory processes.

Taken together, the data from animal studies strongly indicate an important role for the vestibular system in spatial cognition. Although the specific pathways through which vestibular information reaches areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, are yet to be fully elucidated, projections from the vestibular nucleus and cerebellum are likely to transmit information via multiple routes, particularly the thalamus (see Figure 4).41 It is speculated that there is a ‘theta pathway’, which involves projections from the brainstem vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) to the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg), then via various nuclei to the medial septum, which releases ACh into the hippocampus; a ‘head direction pathway’, from the VNC via head direction cells of the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (ADN) to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), to the hippocampus; a major thalamic pathway which transmits vestibular information to the parietal cortex and then on to the MEC and hippocampus; and a transcerebellar pathway.41–43 The detailed pathways are depicted in Figure 4.

Figure 4: ADN, anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; Interpositus N, anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei; LMN, lateral mammillary nuclei; MEC, medial entorhinal cortex; MG, medial geniculate nucleus; NPH, nucleus prepositus hypoglossi; Parietal C, Parietal cortex; PaS, parasubiculum; Perirhinal, Perirhinal cortex; PoS, posterior subiculum (i.e dorsal part of the presubiculum); Post HT, posterior hypothalamus; Postrhinal, postrhinal cortex; PPTg , pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Pulv, pulvinar; RPO, reticularis pontis oralis; SUM, supramammillary nucleus; ViM, ventralis intermedius nuclei of the thalamus; VLN, ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VNC, vestibular nucleus complex; VPi, ventral posterior inferior nucleus of the thalamus; VPL, ventral posterior lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPM, ventral posterior medial nuclei of the thalamus. From Hitier et al.41 with permission.

Vestibular contributions to cognitive function in humans

Consistent with the studies in animals, many studies conducted over the last two decades have demonstrated that vestibular disorders are associated with the impairment of cognitive function in otherwise normal healthy adults.44–53 The studies reviewed were identified using a Pubmed search between 1989 and 2020, using ‘vestibular’ and ‘cognition’ as key words. All of the studies were included; they consisted of two main types: survey and epidemiological studies; and clinical experimental studies (Table 2). Among the symptoms that have been reported are difficulty concentrating, deficits in attention and spatial memory, verbal fluency, mental rotation, and dyscalculia and other forms of numerical cognition (see Table 2). Although some 51 such studies have been published since 1989, only a subset of them (21) have controlled for hearing loss, either by excluding patients with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in the analysis of the data.54–69 Table 2 provides a summary of the studies that have controlled for hearing loss and therefore where the deficits can be considered to be mainly vestibular in origin. Dobbels et al70 have argued that few of the studies reporting cognitive deficits in humans with vestibular disorders, have controlled for hearing loss. However, a careful review of the literature suggests that is not the case. Of course, there are cases in which vestibular and auditory symptoms present together, for example, in Meniere’s disease, in which case they are both likely to contribute to cognitive deficits. Beyond controlling for hearing loss, the major weaknesses of the epidemiological studies are that they often include ‘heterogeneous vestibular disorders’ (bilateral vestibular loss, unilateral vestibular loss, vestibular neuritis, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), etc.) and do not include the same controls as the experimental studies (see Table 2). The available experimental studies are based on samples of patients with different vestibular disorders, but each one of them includes a sample of patients which is compared to a control group without vestibular dysfunction (see Table 2).

Table 2: Studies conducted in humans that have reported cognitive deficits associated with different types of vestibular dysfunction, in which hearing loss has been controlled for in some way, either by excluding subjects with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in a multiple logistic regression model. Where ‘No ()’ occurs, the first number in the brackets indicates the number of subjects without hearing loss and the second, the total sample size.

Abbreviations: UVL, unilateral vestibular loss; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; UVN, unilateral vestibular neurectomy; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; RT, reaction time.

Studies in humans which have combined structural and functional neuro-imaging with behavioural assays, have also provided compelling evidence that vestibular sensory inputs are important for human spatial cognition. In a seminal study of patients with neurofibromatosis type 2 (NF2) who underwent bilateral vestibular nerve section, it was observed that the NF2 patients exhibited significantly poorer spatial navigation skills, measured using a virtual Morris Water Maze Task, which required no movement other than that of a mouse to control a cursor on a computer screen. These spatial cognitive deficits were correlated with reduced hippocampal volumes (approximately 17%) compared to age- and sex-matched controls.51 Only one of these patients exhibited total hearing loss post-operatively and all of them were 8–10 years post-BVL. Subsequent studies have provided further evidence of impaired spatial memory and hippocampal atrophy in patients with other vestibular disorders such as Meniere’s disease.52,71–74 A recent study of over 100 healthy adults reported that poorer vestibular function was correlated with significantly reduced hippocampal volume.75 In our most recent study, we have found that age is statistically related to a bilateral decrease in the volume of the hippocampus and the left entorhinal cortex.76

Vestibular dysfunction as a risk factor for dementia

As a result of the animal and human studies demonstrating that peripheral vestibular lesions caused spatial memory deficits, Previc77 suggested that loss of vestibular function might be implicated in the development of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). His argument was based partly on the limitations of the β-amyloid (Aβ) hypothesis of AD, but the idea can be considered independently of that hypothesis. The central vestibular system, including the brainstem VNC, contributes to major cholinergic inputs to the hippocampus, which is damaged in AD.78 Bilateral vestibular loss in rats also results in a decrease in acetylcholine (ACh) receptors in the hippocampus.38

There have been only a few studies that have directly investigated the relationship between vestibular function and AD, all of them conducted by the same group. The first studies investigated the statistical relationship between vestibular dysfunction and clinical syndromes of cognitive impairment, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. Harun et al79 investigated the prevalence of vestibular dysfunction in 32 patients with AD and 15 with MCI, compared to 94 controls, and estimated that patients with bilaterally absent cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMPs, a measure of saccular function), had a greater than three-fold increased odds of AD (OR 3.42, 95% CI 1.32–8.91, P=0.011). Furthermore, a 1 µV increase in cVEMP amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.28, 95% CI 0.09–0.93, P=0.038). Higher ocular VEMP (oVEMP, indicative of utricular function) amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.92, 95% CI 0.85–0.99, P=0.036). However, there was no significant difference between the MCI group and the controls. Importantly, there was no significant association between VOR function and AD, indicating that semi-circular canal function was not implicated, only the otoliths, the most primitive part of the vestibular system.

In a follow-up study, Wei et al80 examined vestibular function in 51 patients with AD, 26 with MCI and 295 matched controls. The cVEMP, the oVEMP and the VOR were all tested. Compared to controls, they found that people with cVEMP impairment had a 3–4 fold increase in the odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.0, 95% CI 1.1–8.5, P=0.04) and those with oVEMP impairment had an almost four-fold increased odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.9, 95% CI 1.4–11.3, P=0.01). Compared to controls, they found that people with impaired cVEMPs had a five-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 5.0, 95% CI 2.0–12.3, P=0.001) and those with abnormal oVEMPs had a greater than four-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 4.2, 95% CI 1.9–9.1, P≤0.001). Importantly, VOR gain (the ratio of head velocity to eye velocity) was not significantly related to group membership.

There are a number of limitations of these studies which must be noted, however. First, hearing loss was not controlled for in these studies of AD and MCI, so it is possible that it may have been a contributing factor. Second, the sample sizes of AD patients were relatively small, n=32 and 51, and therefore the studies need to be replicated.79,80 Third, the studies were cross-sectional, and the samples may not have been representative of a broader population.80 In particular, patients with both vestibular and cognitive impairment may have been more likely to present than those with either condition alone, resulting in a potential overestimation of the proportion of vestibular dysfunction in AD (‘Berksonian bias’).80 Fourth, it is conceivable that the poor performance by AD patients on the cVEMP and oVEMP testing could have been due to their inability to understand and follow instructions; however, the authors reported that this was not the case.79,80 Finally, the relationship between cVEMP/oVEMP function and AD was a statistical one involving logistic regression and does not necessarily indicate a causal relationship.79,80 For example, aside from the possibility that vestibular dysfunction contributed to the development of AD, it is possible that AD pathology might have caused vestibular dysfunction.

One potential explanation for the relationship between vestibular dysfunction and AD might be that AD pathology (eg, β-amyloid (Aβ)) extends into the central vestibular pathways from the vestibular nucleus to the thalamus and beyond, thereby impairing vestibular function. Although there have been no specific studies of Aβ deposition in ‘vestibular-related areas’ of the brain, vestibular information is distributed widely,41 (see Figure 4), therefore it is likely that AD pathology extends to many brain regions receiving vestibular input. However, in a recent study of vestibular function in 98 participants aged 77.3 (±8.26) from the BLSA, Aβ deposition was measured using amyloid C-11 Pittsburgh Compound B (11C-PB).81 The authors found that 22.4% of the sample were positive for PiB; however, there was no statistically significant relationship between the extent of Aβ deposition and any measure of vestibular function. This study was designed to investigate preclinical AD, but no such study has been performed in patients diagnosed with AD. Another possible explanation is that vestibular impairment directly contributes to medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration and AD, possibly as a result of reduced vestibular sensory input to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, as occurs for auditory input.82

Further studies have concentrated on whether vestibular loss is associated with specific phenotypes of AD, especially those with spatial cognitive deficits. Some AD phenotypes are characterised by predominantly amnestic symptoms compared to others which are characterised by more motoric and spatial impairment.83 In a study of 50 patients with MCI or AD, Wei et al84 observed that patients with vestibular loss were significantly more likely to exhibit impairment in neurocognitive tests of spatial skills, for example the Money Road Map test (MRMT). When patients were divided into ‘spatially normal’ and ‘spatially impaired’ groups based on their performance in the MRMT, only 25% of the spatially normal patients were found to have vestibular dysfunction compared to 96% of the spatially impaired patients.84 In a further study of 60 patients with MCI or AD, patients with vestibular dysfunction were significantly more likely to have difficulty driving, an activity closely linked to spatial cognitive ability.85 It is possible, therefore, that vestibular dysfunction contributes to the development of a ‘spatial’ subtype of AD, increasing the probability of symptoms such as spatial disorientation, wandering, and an increased risk of falling.84 However, these two studies have similar limitations to the original one by Harun et al:79 there were no controls for hearing loss, the sample sizes were relatively small, the studies were cross-sectional and in Wei et al,85 the postural measurements were not specific to vestibular function. Finally, the statistical association reported does not indicate causality.

The only available study that provides any evidence that vestibular loss might be causally involved in the development of cognitive impairment and dementia is by Liao et al86 They investigated prior medical conditions that were associated with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia (LOAD) using a population-based matched case control study based on the National Health Insurance Research database of Taiwan and the Catastrophic Illness Certificate database, between the years 1997 and 2013. The definitions of prior diseases were based on the first three digits of the International Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM). The total case group consisted of 4,600 patients who were newly diagnosed with LOAD between 2007 and 2013, who were then matched to 4,600 controls by both age and sex. Using multivariate logistic regression and path analysis, the authors reported that the incidence of LOAD was positively correlated with prior anxiety (ICD code 300), functional digestive disorder (ICD code 564), psychopathology-specific symptoms (ICD code 307), disorders of the vestibular system (ICD code 386), concussion (ICD code 850), disorders of the urethra and urinary tract (ICD code 599), disorders of refraction and accommodation (ICD code 367) and hearing loss (ICD code 389). While the authors conclude that these data suggest that vestibular dysfunction may therefore be a risk factor for LOAD, the limitations of the study include limited information regarding other confounding factors such as body mass index, blood pressure, diet, smoking, diabetic therapy etc.; and the specific nature of the diagnosis may have varied according to factors affecting access to a neurologist. At present, there are no comparable data available on potential vestibular contributions to dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease or fronto-temporal dementia.

One of the intriguing aspects of the studies in cognitively-normal and vestibular-impaired adults is the demonstration of a link between saccular function and cognition.45,47,69,75,76,79,80,85 We have recently reported that saccular function is a statistically significant predictor of the decrease in hippocampal volume that occurs with age.76 The saccule, one of the two otoliths, is the part of the peripheral vestibular system which detects the orientation of the head with respect to gravity and, together with the utricle, is the oldest component of the vestibular system in evolutionary terms (see Figure 2). Patients with AD exhibited specifically poorer saccular and utricular function compared to age-matched controls.79,85 Saccular stimulation has been demonstrated to activate the multisensory vestibular cortex involved in spatial information processing.87,88 In guinea pigs and rats, selective electrical stimulation of the utricle and saccule has been shown to cause widespread activation of the hippocampus.89,90 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths, the saccule in particular, have a critical role in spatial memory due to their importance in the perception of gravitational vertical.19 We have recently reported that mice lacking otolithic function from birth, exhibit major developmental delays by post-natal day 9, including spatial memory deficits.91 Interestingly, considerable electrophysiological evidence suggests that the neurons in the VNC that subserve the VOR are separate from those involved in the VSR pathways and the pathways to the limbic system and neocortex.92 This means that it is possible for the vestibular pathways that give rise to the conscious perception of self-movement and contribute to spatial memory, to be compromised, without VOR deficits necessarily being exhibited, and that only VSRs such as VEMPs would indicate a vestibular deficit. Figure 5 summarises the hypothesis that saccular dysfunction, in particular, might contribute to the development of cognitive dysfunction that preferentially includes spatial cognitive deficits.

Figure 5: Conceptual model of impact of aging on vestibular function (notably saccular function), which contributes to neurodegeneration of neural circuits involved in vestibular processing and deterioration, specifically in spatial cognitive ability. From Agrawal et al28 with permission.

Conclusions

The available evidence suggests that vestibular dysfunction, including that associated with age-related vestibular loss, has a significant negative impact on cognitive function. Vestibular impairment may therefore be a risk factor for the development of dementia, including AD. Previc93 has recently suggested that, given the increased prevalence of vestibular disorders in females,94 vestibular dysfunction may contribute to their increased incidence of AD. However, the majority of the evidence to date is correlational; therefore, caution must be exercised in interpreting these findings. The potential combined effects of both hearing loss and vestibular loss are unknown but could be expected to be much greater. Of course, it is important to note that some otological disorders involve both auditory and vestibular symptoms (eg, Meniere’s disease). Understanding the full implications of vestibular dysfunction for cognitive decline is potentially of great importance for the health of the elderly, since effective therapies are available to treat vestibular disorders.95 One of the principal treatments for vestibular impairment is vestibular rehabilitation, a suite of physical therapy-based exercises in which head movements are used to stimulate the vestibular system and gradually encourage the brain to adapt to the loss of normal vestibular function.96 Several studies have reported that vestibular rehabilitation can improve cognitive function in healthy adults and in patients with intractable dizziness.67,97 However, at present, studies from the US indicate that only a small number of people with AD are referred for vestibular rehabilitation.98 Since vestibular impairment may be a modifiable risk factor for dementia, the impact of vestibular loss on cognition should be considered along with hearing loss as a critical area for research.

Summary

Abstract

Dementia is recognised to be one of the most challenging diseases facing society, both now and in the future, with its prevalence estimated to increase substantially by 2050. The potential contributions of age-related sensory deficits have attracted little attention until recently, when a landmark study suggested that hearing loss could be a greater risk factor for dementia than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity or social isolation. Over the last decade, evidence has been gradually accumulating to suggest that the other part of the inner ear, the balance organs or ‘vestibular system’, might also be important in the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Increasing evidence suggests that dizziness associated with vestibular dysfunction, a common reason for patients consulting their GPs, increases the risk of cognitive dysfunction, including dementia, and our understanding of the basic neurobiology of this sensory system supports this view. This paper aims to review and critically evaluate the relevant evidence.

Aim

Method

Results

Conclusion

Author Information

Paul F Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the three anonymous referees for their constructive and helpful comments on the manuscript.

Correspondence

Prof Paul Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Correspondence Email

paul.smith@otago.ac.nz

Competing Interests

Nil.

1. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Merfeld D. Dizziness, imbalance and age-related vestibular loss. In: Straka H, editor, Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier, NY, 2020.

2. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, et al. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the national health and nutrition examination survey, 2001-2004. Arch. Intern. Med. 2009; 169(10):938–944.

3. Health Quality and Safety Commission New Zealand, 2020. http://public.tableau.com/profile/hqi2803#!/vizhome/FallsFracturesOutcomesFramework/Landing

4. Hartholt KA, Lee R, Burns ER, Van Beeck EF. Mortality from falls among US adults aged 75 years or older, 2000–2016. JAMA. 2019; 321(21):2131–2133.

5. Agrawal Y, Merfeld D, Horak F, et al. Aging, vestibular function and balance: Proceedings of a National Institute on Aging/National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders Workshop. J. Gerontol.: Med. Sci. 2020, in press.

6. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ, Angelaki DE, et al. The Vestibular System: A Sixth Sense. New York: Oxford University Press, 2012.

7. Curthoys IS, Grant JW, Burgess AM, et al. Otolith receptor mechanisms for vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials: A review. Front. Neurol. 2018; 9:366.

8. Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang Y, et al. Anatomy and surgical approach to rat’s vestibular sensors and nerves. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2016; 270:1–8.

9. Urciuoli A, Zanolli C, Beaudet A, et al. The evolution of the vestibular apparatus in apes and humans. Elife. 2020; 9. pii:e51261.

10. Ramos de Miguel A, Zarowski A, Sluydts M, et al. The superiority of the otolith system.  Audiol Neurootol. 2020; 25(1–2):35–41.

11. Stackman RW, Clark AS, Taube JS. Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input. Hippocampus 2002; 12:291–303.

12. Russell NA, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Long-term effects of permanent vestibular lesions on hippocampal spatial firing. J. Neurosci. 2003; 23(16):6490–6498.

13. Jacob P-Y, Poucet B, Liberge M, et al. Vestibular control of entorhinal cortex activity in spatial navigation. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:38.

14. Besnard S, Lopez C, Brandt T, et al. (editors) The vestibular system in cognitive and memory processes in mammals. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. Lausanne: Frontiers Media. doi: 10.3389/978-2-88919-744-6 (ebook) pp. 1–246, 2016.

15. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2017; 30(1):84–89.

16. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971; 34(1):171–5.

17. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, et al. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2015; 436(7052):801–6.

18. Moser EI, Moser MB, McNaughton BL. Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1448–1464.

19. Smith PF. The growing evidence for the importance of the otoliths for spatial memory. Front. Neural Circ. 2019; 3(66):1–14.

20. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017; 390(10113):2673–2734.

21. Bathini P, Brai E, Auber LA. Olfactory dysfunction in the pathophysiological continuum of dementia. Ageing Res Rev. 2019; 55:100956.

22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntly J, et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. The Lancet Global Health. 2019; 7:e596–e603.

23. Michalowsky B, Hoffmann W, Kostev K. Association between hearing and vision impairment and risk of dementia: Results of a case-control study based on secondary data. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 20;11: 363.

24. Golub JS, Brickman AM, Ciarleglio AJ, et al. Association of subclinical hearing loss with cognitive performance. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2020; 146(1):57–67.

25. Loughrey DG, Parra MA, Lawlor BA. Visual short-term memory binding deficit with age-related hearing loss in cognitively normal older adults. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):12600.

26. Liu CM, Lee CT. Association of hearing loss with dementia. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(7): e198112.

27. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, et al. Longitudinal study of self-reported hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in women. Alzheimers Dement. 2020; 16(4):610–620.

28. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Rosenberg PB. Vestibular impairment, cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s Disease: Balancing the evidence. Aging and Mental Health. 2020; 24(5):705–708.

29. Smith PF, Zheng Y. From ear to uncertainty: Vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2013; 7:84.

30. Smith PF, Geddes LH, Baek J-H, et al. Modulation of cognitive function by vestibular lesions and galvanic vestibular stimulation. Front. Neurol. 2010; 1(141):1–8.

31. Smith PF, Darlington CL, Zheng Y. The effects of complete vestibular deafferentation on spatial memory and the hippocampus in rat: The Dunedin experience. In: Ferre, E.R, Harris, L. (editors), Vestibular Cognition. Brill, Leiden, 2017; pp. 461–485.

32. Baek JH, Zheng Y, Darlington C L, et al. Evidence that spatial memory deficits following bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats are probably permanent. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2010; 94(3):402–413.

33. Russell N, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2006; 96:4–14.

34. Neo P, Carter, D, Zheng, Y, et al. Septal elicitation of hippocampal theta rhythm did not repair the cognitive and emotional deficits resulting from vestibular lesions. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:1176–1187.

35. Tai SK, Ma J, Ossenkopp KP, et al. Activation of immobility-related hippocampal theta by cholinergic septohippocampal neurons during vestibular stimulation. Hippocampus. 2012; 22(4):914–25.

36. Cullen KE, Taube JS. Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges.  Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1465–1473.

37. Harvey RE, Rutan SA, Willey GR, et al. M. Linear self-motion cues support the spatial distribution and stability of hippocampal place cells. Curr. Biol. 2018; 28(11):1810.e5.

38. Aitken P, Benoit A, Zheng Y, et al. Hippocampal and striatal M1-muscarinic acetylcholine receptors are down-regulated following bilateral vestibular loss in rats. Hippocampus. 2016; 26:1509–1514.

39. Besnard S, Machado ML, Vignaux G, et al. Influence of vestibular input on spatial and nonspatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:814–826.

40. Truchet B, Benoit A, Chaillan F, et al. Hippocampal LTP modulation and glutamatergic receptors following vestibular loss. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019; 224:699–711.

41. Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:59.

42. Bohne P, Schwarz MK, Herlitze S, Mark MD. A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Front Neural Circuits. 2019; 13:51.

43. Watson TC, Obiang P, Torres-Herraez A, et al. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife. 2019; 8:e41896.

44. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Du Lac S, et al. Vestibular vertigo and comorbid cognitive and psychiatric impairment: The 2008 national health interview survey. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. 2016; 87(4):310–319.

45. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Trevino C, et al. Association between visuo- spatial ability and vestibular function in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 2015; 63(9):1837–1844.

46. Semenov YR, Bigelow RT, Xue Q, et al. Association between vestibular and cognitive function in US adults: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J. Gerontol. Series A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015; 71(2):243–250.

47. Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, et al. Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: Data from the Triangle Completion Task. Front Neurol. 2017; 8:173.

48. Wackym PA, Balaban CD, Mackay HT, et al. Longitudinal cognitive and neurobehavioral functional outcomes before and after repairing otic capsule dehiscence. Otol Neurotol. 2016; 37(1):70–82.

49. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol. 2017; 264(3):554–563.

50. Smith L, Wilkinson D, Bodani M, et al. Short-term memory impairment in vestibular patients can arise independently of psychiatric impairment, fatigue, and sleeplessness. J Neuropsychol. 2019; 13(3):417–431.

51. Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. 2005; 128(11):2732–2741.

52. Kremmyda O, Huffner K, Flanagin VL, et al. Beyond dizziness: Virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front. Human Neurosci. 2016; 10:139.

53. Ayar DA, Kumral E, Celebisoy N. Cognitive functions in acute vestibular loss. J. Neurol. 2020, in press. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w.

54. Sang FY, Jáuregui-Renaud K, Green DA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms in vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006; 77(6):760–6.

55. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang FY, Gresty MA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008; 79(3):276–83.

56. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolaños M, et al. Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res. 2008; 18(2–3):129–37.

57. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Hoffman HJ, et al. Association between vertigo, cognitive and psychiatric conditions in US children: 2012 National Health Interview Survey. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2020; 130:109802.

58. Risey J, Briner W. Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J. Vest. Res. 1990; 1:31–37.

59. Redfern MS, Talkowski ME, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive influences on postural control of patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Gait and Posture. 2004; 19:105–114.

60. Talkowski ME, Redfern MS, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive requirements for vestibular and ocular motor processing in healthy adults and patients with unilateral vestibular lesions. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2005; 17(9):1432–1441.

61. Gomez-Alvarez FB, Jáuregui-Renaud K. Psychological symptoms and spatial orientation during the first 3 months after acute vestibular neuritis. Arch. Med. Res. 2011; 42:97–103.

62. Caixeta GC dos S, Dona F, Gazzola JM. Cognitive processing and body balance in elderly subjects with vestibular dysfunction. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2012; 78(2):87–95.

63. Candidi M, Micarelli A, Viziano A, et al. Impaired mental rotation in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and acute vestibular neuritis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013; 7:783.

64. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Acute peripheral vestibular deficit increases the redundancy in random number generation. Exp. Brain Res. 2017; 235:627–637.

65. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Impaired math achievement in patients with acute vestibular neuritis. Neuropsychologia. 2017; 107:1–8.

66. Lofti Y, Rezazadeh N, Moossavi A, et al. Preliminary evidence of improved cognitive performance following vestibular rehabilitation in children with combined ADHD (cADHD) and concurrent vestibular impairment. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2017; 44:700–707.

67. Sugaya N, Arai M, Goto F. Changes in cognitive function in patients with intractable dizziness following vestibular rehabilitation. Sci. Reps. 2018; 8:9984.

68. Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, et al. Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs embodied spatial cognition. J. Neurol. 2019; 266(Suppl. 1: S149–S159.

69. Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei et al. Association between saccule and semi-circular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear. 2020; 41:686–692.

70. Dobbels B, Peetermans O, Boon B, et al. Impact of bilateral vestibulopathy on spatial and nonspatial cognition: A systematic review. Ear Hear. 2018; 40(4):757–765.

71. zu Eulenburg P, Stoeter P, Dieterich M. Voxel-based morphometry depicts central compensation after vestibular neuritis. Annal. Neurol. 2010; 68:241–249.

72. Van Cruijsen N, Hiemstra WM, Meiners LC, et al. Hippocampal volume measurement in patients with Ménière’s disease: a pilot study. Acta Otolaryngol. 2007; 127(10):1018–23.

73. Seo YJ, Kim J, Kim SH. The change of hippocampal volume and its relevance with inner ear function in Meniere’s disease patients. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2016; 43(6):620–5.

74. Göttlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, et al. Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016; 37(5):1998–2006.

75. Kamil RJ, Jacob A, Ratnanather JT, et al. Vestibular function and hippocampal volume in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA). Otol. Neurotol. 2018; (6):765–771.

76. Jacob A, Tward D, Resnick S, et al. Vestibular function and cortical and sub-cortical alterations in an aging population. Heliyon. 2020, 6: e04728.

77. Previc FH. Vestibular loss as a contributor to Alzheimer’s disease. Med. Hypoth. 2013; 80(4):360–367.

78. Chapleau M, Aldebert J, Montembeault M, Brambati SM. Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease and Semantic Dementia: An ALE Meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016; 54(3):941–955.

79. Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow RT, et al. Vestibular impairment in dementia. Otol. Neurotol. 2016; 37(8):1137–1142.

80. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Vestibular loss predicts poorer spatial cognition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017; 61(3):995–1003.

81. Kamil RJ, Bilgel M, Wong DF, et al. Vestibular function and beta-amyloid deposition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.  Front Aging Neurosci. 2018; 10:408.

82. Cui B, Zhu L, She X, et al. Chronic noise exposure causes persistence of tau hyperphosphorylation and formation of NFT tau in the rat hippocampus and pre- frontal cortex. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 238(2):122–129.

83. Mapstone M, Steffenella TM, Duffy CJ. A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment: getting lost between aging and AD. Neurol. 2003; 60(5):802–808.

84. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Increased prevalence of vestibular loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2019; 16(12):1143–1150.

85. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Saccular impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with driving difficulty. Dement. Geriatric Cog. Dis. 2017; 44(5–6):294–302.

86. Liao JY, Lee CT, Lin TY, et al. Exploring prior diseases associated with incident late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia. PLoS One. 2020; 15(1):e0228172.

87. Miyamoto T, Fukushima K, Takada T, et al. Saccular projections in the human cerebral cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2005; 1039(1):124–131.

88. Schlindwein P, Mueller M, Bauermann T, et al. Cortical representation of saccular vestibular stimulation: VEMPs in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2008; 39(1):19–31.

89. Cuthbert PC, Gilchrist DP, Hicks SL, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for vestibular activation of the guinea pig hippocampus. Neuroreport. 2000; 11(7):1443–47.

90. Smith PF, Hitier M, Zhang, Y-F, et al. Vestibular modulation of hippocampal and striatal function. J. Vest. Res. 2019; 29(1):18.

91. Le Gall A, Hilber P, Chesneau C, et al. The critical role of vestibular graviception during cognitive-motor development. Behav. Brain Res. 2019; 372:112040.

92. Cullen KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control.  Trends Neurosci. 2012; 35(3):185–96.

93. Previc F. Does vestibular dysfunction contribute to the increased prevalence of Alzheimer’s Disease in females? Clin. Case. Revs. 2019; 2(1):1–2.

94. Smith PF, Agrawal Y, Darlington CL. Sexual dimorphism in vestibular function and dysfunction. J. Neurophysiol. 2019; 121:2379–2391.

95. Hillier S, McDonnell M. Is vestibular rehabilitation effective in improving dizziness and function after unilateral peripheral vestibular hypofunction? an abridged version of a Cochrane Review. Eur. J. Phys. Rehab. Med. 2016; 52(4):541–556.

96. Herdman SJ, Clendaniel R. Vestibular Rehabilitation (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis. 2014.

97. Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech A, et al. Balance training improves memory and spatial cognition in healthy adults.  Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1):5661.

98. Gandhi P, Klatt BN, Agrawal Y. Physical and vestibular physical therapy referrals in people with Alzheimer Disease.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000390.

For the PDF of this article,
contact nzmj@nzma.org.nz

View Article PDF

Dizziness is reported to be one of the most common reasons for patients consulting a general practitioner.1 Although not all consultations regarding dizziness are related to the vestibular system, for example, they can be due to cardiovascular dysfunction, approximately 20–50% are believed to be due to balance disorders related to the peripheral vestibular system (see Table 1).1 Recent studies have estimated that 35% of US adults over the age of 35 years suffer from vestibular disorders, increasing to 85% aged 80 and over.2 Unfortunately, no reliable statistics are available on the prevalence of vestibular disorders for New Zealand. However, the Health Quality and Safety Commission reported that even between January and March, 2020, there were 65,893 fall injuries reported to the ACC, of which 8,544 were classified as serious.3 Overseas, impairment of the vestibular system has been estimated to increase the odds of falling by over 12-fold, and nearly 30% of adults aged 65 and over fall each year.2,4,5 In 2016, the mortality rate associated with falls in the US was estimated to be 122.2 per 100,000 persons.4,5

Table 1: Common causes of dizziness in primary care practice. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

The impact of vestibular dysfunction on loss of balance and falls is due both to its effects on brainstem vestibular reflex pathways as well as higher cognitive processing of self-motion signals from the vestibular system. The vestibular system (Figure 1) senses head movement (strictly speaking ‘head acceleration’ or change in head velocity) in different planes, as well as linear acceleration by gravity.6 The three semi-circular canals in each inner ear sense angular rotation of the head, and the two otoliths, the utricle and the saccule, sense linear movement.7,8 This linear movement not only includes movement of the head, forward and backward, and left and right, but linear acceleration of the head by gravity; the saccule, in particular, senses linear acceleration by gravity (Figure 2 for explanation).7 The utricle and saccule detect linear acceleration as a result of ‘otoconia’ (calcium carbonate crystals) that generate an inertial force on the hair cells during head movement, causing a change in electrical potential (see Figure 2).7 In this respect, it is important to note that the most primitive form of the otoliths (‘statoliths’) are estimated to have evolved approximately 670 million years ago, and they exist in invertebrates such as jellyfish. This is their only means of detecting upright, which is necessary for survival. Therefore, given their evolutionary age, the otoliths might be expected to have developed major contributions to balance in humans.9,10 The vestibular system, through short-latency brainstem pathways, generates rapid eye movements that compensate for the unintentional movement of the head, eg, movement of the head due to the pulse beat (the vestibulo-ocular reflexes or VORs) and maintains the stability of the visual image of the world on the retina.6 The vestibular system also generates rapid vestibulo-spinal reflexes (VSRs) which adjust posture for unintentional movement, enabling us to keep our balance.6 Without a normal vestibular system, vision would become blurred (a condition known as ‘oscillopsia’) and balance and locomotion become disrupted.6

Figure 1: Human inner ear anatomy. The cochlea and vestibular system, which encompasses the three semicircular canals (horizontal, superior (anterior vertical) and posterior vertical) and the two otolith organs (utricle and saccule), are depicted. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Figure 2: (A) Schematic representation of the plates of the otolithic receptors (the utricular and saccular maculae). The arrows show the preferred polarization of the hair cell receptors across the maculae. The dashed lines are lines of polarity reversal (lpr). The striola refers to a band of receptors on either side of the lpr. Schematics of type I (B,D) and type II receptors (C,E) show how linear acceleration acts on the otoliths and so deflects the hair bundles of individual receptors. From Curthoys et al7 with permission.

Information about angular and linear head movement is also transmitted to higher centres of the brain, where it contributes to the conscious experience of moving through the environment and to cognitive processes such as memory.11–15 As we move through the environment, the vestibular hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths detect every head movement, and transmit this information to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, where it is assimilated and stored to provide a spatial map of our movements.11–15 This information is integrated with other sensory information, such as that from the visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory and proprioceptive systems, and formulated into mathematical maps of the spatial world, allowing us to navigate through it more effectively.11–15 In the 1970s, specific neurons were discovered in the hippocampus that selectively discharged in response to specific areas of the environment. These became known as ‘place cells’.16 In the 1990s, related cells were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex, known as ‘grid cells’, which discharged in response to multiple areas in the environment.17 In 2014, the Nobel Prize in Medicine or Physiology was awarded to John O’Keefe, Edvard Moser and Britt-Mayer Moser for these discoveries, which have become known as the brain’s ‘global positioning system’.18 Since then, both place cells and grid cells have been demonstrated to rely on vestibular information from the inner ear.11–13 Therefore, one reason why vestibular-related dizziness contributes to falls, is that not only does it impair fast vestibular reflexes such as the VORs and VSRs, but it impairs the ability of the brain to integrate self-motion information and to navigate through the spatial environment and form spatial memories. Information from the vestibular system is distributed widely throughout the central nervous system and is involved in higher cognitive function.11–15,18 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths may be important for cognitive processing independently of the semi-circular canals;19 this is one reason why the evolutionary age of the otoliths is of interest.

In recent years, a substantial amount of epidemiological evidence has been published to support the idea that age-related hearing loss is a risk factor for dementia.20 For example, in a seminal study published in the Lancet, it was reported that the contribution of hearing loss to the incidence of dementia was greater than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity and social isolation.20 This result seemed surprising, because sensory systems had never been considered particularly important to dementia, except perhaps for olfactory function as a potential biomarker.21 It is important to note that the Livingstone et al study20 was based on data from high-income countries and the evidence from low-to-middle income countries is less convincing in this respect.22 Over the last several years, further evidence in support of the importance of hearing loss for the development of dementia has been published, although it is not entirely consistent.22–27

Less attention has been given to the other part of the inner ear, the vestibular system (see Figure 1); however, evidence is mounting that age-related vestibular disorders could also be a significant risk factor for the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia, along with hearing loss.28 The vestibular system is known to degenerate with age, as with other sensory systems, with decreases in hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths, a reduction in the number of neurons in the vestibular nerve and brainstem vestibular nucleus, and a deterioration of vestibular reflex responses.1,2 As shown in the Iceberg model in Figure 3, clinical presentation of age-related vestibular symptoms usually includes ‘presbystasis’ (the imbalance of disequilibrium) or ‘presbyvertigo’ (vertigo), or both, possibly with a decrease in vestibular perception; however, it is possible for age-related vestibular symptoms to be sub-clinical and therefore harder to detect .

Figure 3: Iceberg model of age-related vestibular loss.

VSR: vestibulo-spinal reflex; VOR: vestibulo-ocular reflex. ‘Presbystasis’: age-related imbalance of disequilibrium. ‘Presbyvertigo’: age-related vertigo. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Animal studies supporting the role of the vestibular system in cognitive function

The literature relating the vestibular system to cognitive function, especially spatial memory, dates back to the 1960’s.14 Numerous studies have reported evidence that unilateral or bilateral lesions of the peripheral vestibular system impair spatial memory in various maze and foraging tasks.14,29–31 In some cases, these have been conducted even 14 months after bilateral vestibular lesions (BVL) in rats, and the spatial memory deficits remain.32 A variety of potentially confounding factors have been controlled for, including vision, degree of motor activity, anxiety and auditory function, and the results have been consistent.29–32 BVL has been demonstrated to impair the function of neurons in the hippocampus that encode places in the environment (‘hippocampal place cells’),11,12 EEG activity in the theta frequency range,33–35 which is thought to regulate place cell function, and theta EEG activity among grid cells of the entorhinal cortex.13 Neurons in the thalamus which encode head direction (‘head direction cells’), are also dysfunctional following BVL.36 Together, these abnormalities in the function of place cells, grid cells and head direction cells, are likely to underlie the spatial memory deficits observed in animals.14,15 Furthermore, transgenic mice without otoconia and therefore without otolith function (‘otolith deficient tilted mice’), but with normal semi-circular canal function, have been shown to have aberrant hippocampal place cell and head direction cell activity.36,37 In addition, a variety of neurochemical changes have been documented in the hippocampus following BVL, including changes in the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype of glutamate receptor and muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptors,38–40 both of which are implicated in hippocampal learning and memory processes.

Taken together, the data from animal studies strongly indicate an important role for the vestibular system in spatial cognition. Although the specific pathways through which vestibular information reaches areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, are yet to be fully elucidated, projections from the vestibular nucleus and cerebellum are likely to transmit information via multiple routes, particularly the thalamus (see Figure 4).41 It is speculated that there is a ‘theta pathway’, which involves projections from the brainstem vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) to the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg), then via various nuclei to the medial septum, which releases ACh into the hippocampus; a ‘head direction pathway’, from the VNC via head direction cells of the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (ADN) to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), to the hippocampus; a major thalamic pathway which transmits vestibular information to the parietal cortex and then on to the MEC and hippocampus; and a transcerebellar pathway.41–43 The detailed pathways are depicted in Figure 4.

Figure 4: ADN, anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; Interpositus N, anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei; LMN, lateral mammillary nuclei; MEC, medial entorhinal cortex; MG, medial geniculate nucleus; NPH, nucleus prepositus hypoglossi; Parietal C, Parietal cortex; PaS, parasubiculum; Perirhinal, Perirhinal cortex; PoS, posterior subiculum (i.e dorsal part of the presubiculum); Post HT, posterior hypothalamus; Postrhinal, postrhinal cortex; PPTg , pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Pulv, pulvinar; RPO, reticularis pontis oralis; SUM, supramammillary nucleus; ViM, ventralis intermedius nuclei of the thalamus; VLN, ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VNC, vestibular nucleus complex; VPi, ventral posterior inferior nucleus of the thalamus; VPL, ventral posterior lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPM, ventral posterior medial nuclei of the thalamus. From Hitier et al.41 with permission.

Vestibular contributions to cognitive function in humans

Consistent with the studies in animals, many studies conducted over the last two decades have demonstrated that vestibular disorders are associated with the impairment of cognitive function in otherwise normal healthy adults.44–53 The studies reviewed were identified using a Pubmed search between 1989 and 2020, using ‘vestibular’ and ‘cognition’ as key words. All of the studies were included; they consisted of two main types: survey and epidemiological studies; and clinical experimental studies (Table 2). Among the symptoms that have been reported are difficulty concentrating, deficits in attention and spatial memory, verbal fluency, mental rotation, and dyscalculia and other forms of numerical cognition (see Table 2). Although some 51 such studies have been published since 1989, only a subset of them (21) have controlled for hearing loss, either by excluding patients with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in the analysis of the data.54–69 Table 2 provides a summary of the studies that have controlled for hearing loss and therefore where the deficits can be considered to be mainly vestibular in origin. Dobbels et al70 have argued that few of the studies reporting cognitive deficits in humans with vestibular disorders, have controlled for hearing loss. However, a careful review of the literature suggests that is not the case. Of course, there are cases in which vestibular and auditory symptoms present together, for example, in Meniere’s disease, in which case they are both likely to contribute to cognitive deficits. Beyond controlling for hearing loss, the major weaknesses of the epidemiological studies are that they often include ‘heterogeneous vestibular disorders’ (bilateral vestibular loss, unilateral vestibular loss, vestibular neuritis, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), etc.) and do not include the same controls as the experimental studies (see Table 2). The available experimental studies are based on samples of patients with different vestibular disorders, but each one of them includes a sample of patients which is compared to a control group without vestibular dysfunction (see Table 2).

Table 2: Studies conducted in humans that have reported cognitive deficits associated with different types of vestibular dysfunction, in which hearing loss has been controlled for in some way, either by excluding subjects with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in a multiple logistic regression model. Where ‘No ()’ occurs, the first number in the brackets indicates the number of subjects without hearing loss and the second, the total sample size.

Abbreviations: UVL, unilateral vestibular loss; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; UVN, unilateral vestibular neurectomy; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; RT, reaction time.

Studies in humans which have combined structural and functional neuro-imaging with behavioural assays, have also provided compelling evidence that vestibular sensory inputs are important for human spatial cognition. In a seminal study of patients with neurofibromatosis type 2 (NF2) who underwent bilateral vestibular nerve section, it was observed that the NF2 patients exhibited significantly poorer spatial navigation skills, measured using a virtual Morris Water Maze Task, which required no movement other than that of a mouse to control a cursor on a computer screen. These spatial cognitive deficits were correlated with reduced hippocampal volumes (approximately 17%) compared to age- and sex-matched controls.51 Only one of these patients exhibited total hearing loss post-operatively and all of them were 8–10 years post-BVL. Subsequent studies have provided further evidence of impaired spatial memory and hippocampal atrophy in patients with other vestibular disorders such as Meniere’s disease.52,71–74 A recent study of over 100 healthy adults reported that poorer vestibular function was correlated with significantly reduced hippocampal volume.75 In our most recent study, we have found that age is statistically related to a bilateral decrease in the volume of the hippocampus and the left entorhinal cortex.76

Vestibular dysfunction as a risk factor for dementia

As a result of the animal and human studies demonstrating that peripheral vestibular lesions caused spatial memory deficits, Previc77 suggested that loss of vestibular function might be implicated in the development of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). His argument was based partly on the limitations of the β-amyloid (Aβ) hypothesis of AD, but the idea can be considered independently of that hypothesis. The central vestibular system, including the brainstem VNC, contributes to major cholinergic inputs to the hippocampus, which is damaged in AD.78 Bilateral vestibular loss in rats also results in a decrease in acetylcholine (ACh) receptors in the hippocampus.38

There have been only a few studies that have directly investigated the relationship between vestibular function and AD, all of them conducted by the same group. The first studies investigated the statistical relationship between vestibular dysfunction and clinical syndromes of cognitive impairment, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. Harun et al79 investigated the prevalence of vestibular dysfunction in 32 patients with AD and 15 with MCI, compared to 94 controls, and estimated that patients with bilaterally absent cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMPs, a measure of saccular function), had a greater than three-fold increased odds of AD (OR 3.42, 95% CI 1.32–8.91, P=0.011). Furthermore, a 1 µV increase in cVEMP amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.28, 95% CI 0.09–0.93, P=0.038). Higher ocular VEMP (oVEMP, indicative of utricular function) amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.92, 95% CI 0.85–0.99, P=0.036). However, there was no significant difference between the MCI group and the controls. Importantly, there was no significant association between VOR function and AD, indicating that semi-circular canal function was not implicated, only the otoliths, the most primitive part of the vestibular system.

In a follow-up study, Wei et al80 examined vestibular function in 51 patients with AD, 26 with MCI and 295 matched controls. The cVEMP, the oVEMP and the VOR were all tested. Compared to controls, they found that people with cVEMP impairment had a 3–4 fold increase in the odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.0, 95% CI 1.1–8.5, P=0.04) and those with oVEMP impairment had an almost four-fold increased odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.9, 95% CI 1.4–11.3, P=0.01). Compared to controls, they found that people with impaired cVEMPs had a five-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 5.0, 95% CI 2.0–12.3, P=0.001) and those with abnormal oVEMPs had a greater than four-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 4.2, 95% CI 1.9–9.1, P≤0.001). Importantly, VOR gain (the ratio of head velocity to eye velocity) was not significantly related to group membership.

There are a number of limitations of these studies which must be noted, however. First, hearing loss was not controlled for in these studies of AD and MCI, so it is possible that it may have been a contributing factor. Second, the sample sizes of AD patients were relatively small, n=32 and 51, and therefore the studies need to be replicated.79,80 Third, the studies were cross-sectional, and the samples may not have been representative of a broader population.80 In particular, patients with both vestibular and cognitive impairment may have been more likely to present than those with either condition alone, resulting in a potential overestimation of the proportion of vestibular dysfunction in AD (‘Berksonian bias’).80 Fourth, it is conceivable that the poor performance by AD patients on the cVEMP and oVEMP testing could have been due to their inability to understand and follow instructions; however, the authors reported that this was not the case.79,80 Finally, the relationship between cVEMP/oVEMP function and AD was a statistical one involving logistic regression and does not necessarily indicate a causal relationship.79,80 For example, aside from the possibility that vestibular dysfunction contributed to the development of AD, it is possible that AD pathology might have caused vestibular dysfunction.

One potential explanation for the relationship between vestibular dysfunction and AD might be that AD pathology (eg, β-amyloid (Aβ)) extends into the central vestibular pathways from the vestibular nucleus to the thalamus and beyond, thereby impairing vestibular function. Although there have been no specific studies of Aβ deposition in ‘vestibular-related areas’ of the brain, vestibular information is distributed widely,41 (see Figure 4), therefore it is likely that AD pathology extends to many brain regions receiving vestibular input. However, in a recent study of vestibular function in 98 participants aged 77.3 (±8.26) from the BLSA, Aβ deposition was measured using amyloid C-11 Pittsburgh Compound B (11C-PB).81 The authors found that 22.4% of the sample were positive for PiB; however, there was no statistically significant relationship between the extent of Aβ deposition and any measure of vestibular function. This study was designed to investigate preclinical AD, but no such study has been performed in patients diagnosed with AD. Another possible explanation is that vestibular impairment directly contributes to medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration and AD, possibly as a result of reduced vestibular sensory input to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, as occurs for auditory input.82

Further studies have concentrated on whether vestibular loss is associated with specific phenotypes of AD, especially those with spatial cognitive deficits. Some AD phenotypes are characterised by predominantly amnestic symptoms compared to others which are characterised by more motoric and spatial impairment.83 In a study of 50 patients with MCI or AD, Wei et al84 observed that patients with vestibular loss were significantly more likely to exhibit impairment in neurocognitive tests of spatial skills, for example the Money Road Map test (MRMT). When patients were divided into ‘spatially normal’ and ‘spatially impaired’ groups based on their performance in the MRMT, only 25% of the spatially normal patients were found to have vestibular dysfunction compared to 96% of the spatially impaired patients.84 In a further study of 60 patients with MCI or AD, patients with vestibular dysfunction were significantly more likely to have difficulty driving, an activity closely linked to spatial cognitive ability.85 It is possible, therefore, that vestibular dysfunction contributes to the development of a ‘spatial’ subtype of AD, increasing the probability of symptoms such as spatial disorientation, wandering, and an increased risk of falling.84 However, these two studies have similar limitations to the original one by Harun et al:79 there were no controls for hearing loss, the sample sizes were relatively small, the studies were cross-sectional and in Wei et al,85 the postural measurements were not specific to vestibular function. Finally, the statistical association reported does not indicate causality.

The only available study that provides any evidence that vestibular loss might be causally involved in the development of cognitive impairment and dementia is by Liao et al86 They investigated prior medical conditions that were associated with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia (LOAD) using a population-based matched case control study based on the National Health Insurance Research database of Taiwan and the Catastrophic Illness Certificate database, between the years 1997 and 2013. The definitions of prior diseases were based on the first three digits of the International Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM). The total case group consisted of 4,600 patients who were newly diagnosed with LOAD between 2007 and 2013, who were then matched to 4,600 controls by both age and sex. Using multivariate logistic regression and path analysis, the authors reported that the incidence of LOAD was positively correlated with prior anxiety (ICD code 300), functional digestive disorder (ICD code 564), psychopathology-specific symptoms (ICD code 307), disorders of the vestibular system (ICD code 386), concussion (ICD code 850), disorders of the urethra and urinary tract (ICD code 599), disorders of refraction and accommodation (ICD code 367) and hearing loss (ICD code 389). While the authors conclude that these data suggest that vestibular dysfunction may therefore be a risk factor for LOAD, the limitations of the study include limited information regarding other confounding factors such as body mass index, blood pressure, diet, smoking, diabetic therapy etc.; and the specific nature of the diagnosis may have varied according to factors affecting access to a neurologist. At present, there are no comparable data available on potential vestibular contributions to dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease or fronto-temporal dementia.

One of the intriguing aspects of the studies in cognitively-normal and vestibular-impaired adults is the demonstration of a link between saccular function and cognition.45,47,69,75,76,79,80,85 We have recently reported that saccular function is a statistically significant predictor of the decrease in hippocampal volume that occurs with age.76 The saccule, one of the two otoliths, is the part of the peripheral vestibular system which detects the orientation of the head with respect to gravity and, together with the utricle, is the oldest component of the vestibular system in evolutionary terms (see Figure 2). Patients with AD exhibited specifically poorer saccular and utricular function compared to age-matched controls.79,85 Saccular stimulation has been demonstrated to activate the multisensory vestibular cortex involved in spatial information processing.87,88 In guinea pigs and rats, selective electrical stimulation of the utricle and saccule has been shown to cause widespread activation of the hippocampus.89,90 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths, the saccule in particular, have a critical role in spatial memory due to their importance in the perception of gravitational vertical.19 We have recently reported that mice lacking otolithic function from birth, exhibit major developmental delays by post-natal day 9, including spatial memory deficits.91 Interestingly, considerable electrophysiological evidence suggests that the neurons in the VNC that subserve the VOR are separate from those involved in the VSR pathways and the pathways to the limbic system and neocortex.92 This means that it is possible for the vestibular pathways that give rise to the conscious perception of self-movement and contribute to spatial memory, to be compromised, without VOR deficits necessarily being exhibited, and that only VSRs such as VEMPs would indicate a vestibular deficit. Figure 5 summarises the hypothesis that saccular dysfunction, in particular, might contribute to the development of cognitive dysfunction that preferentially includes spatial cognitive deficits.

Figure 5: Conceptual model of impact of aging on vestibular function (notably saccular function), which contributes to neurodegeneration of neural circuits involved in vestibular processing and deterioration, specifically in spatial cognitive ability. From Agrawal et al28 with permission.

Conclusions

The available evidence suggests that vestibular dysfunction, including that associated with age-related vestibular loss, has a significant negative impact on cognitive function. Vestibular impairment may therefore be a risk factor for the development of dementia, including AD. Previc93 has recently suggested that, given the increased prevalence of vestibular disorders in females,94 vestibular dysfunction may contribute to their increased incidence of AD. However, the majority of the evidence to date is correlational; therefore, caution must be exercised in interpreting these findings. The potential combined effects of both hearing loss and vestibular loss are unknown but could be expected to be much greater. Of course, it is important to note that some otological disorders involve both auditory and vestibular symptoms (eg, Meniere’s disease). Understanding the full implications of vestibular dysfunction for cognitive decline is potentially of great importance for the health of the elderly, since effective therapies are available to treat vestibular disorders.95 One of the principal treatments for vestibular impairment is vestibular rehabilitation, a suite of physical therapy-based exercises in which head movements are used to stimulate the vestibular system and gradually encourage the brain to adapt to the loss of normal vestibular function.96 Several studies have reported that vestibular rehabilitation can improve cognitive function in healthy adults and in patients with intractable dizziness.67,97 However, at present, studies from the US indicate that only a small number of people with AD are referred for vestibular rehabilitation.98 Since vestibular impairment may be a modifiable risk factor for dementia, the impact of vestibular loss on cognition should be considered along with hearing loss as a critical area for research.

Summary

Abstract

Dementia is recognised to be one of the most challenging diseases facing society, both now and in the future, with its prevalence estimated to increase substantially by 2050. The potential contributions of age-related sensory deficits have attracted little attention until recently, when a landmark study suggested that hearing loss could be a greater risk factor for dementia than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity or social isolation. Over the last decade, evidence has been gradually accumulating to suggest that the other part of the inner ear, the balance organs or ‘vestibular system’, might also be important in the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Increasing evidence suggests that dizziness associated with vestibular dysfunction, a common reason for patients consulting their GPs, increases the risk of cognitive dysfunction, including dementia, and our understanding of the basic neurobiology of this sensory system supports this view. This paper aims to review and critically evaluate the relevant evidence.

Aim

Method

Results

Conclusion

Author Information

Paul F Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the three anonymous referees for their constructive and helpful comments on the manuscript.

Correspondence

Prof Paul Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Correspondence Email

paul.smith@otago.ac.nz

Competing Interests

Nil.

1. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Merfeld D. Dizziness, imbalance and age-related vestibular loss. In: Straka H, editor, Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier, NY, 2020.

2. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, et al. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the national health and nutrition examination survey, 2001-2004. Arch. Intern. Med. 2009; 169(10):938–944.

3. Health Quality and Safety Commission New Zealand, 2020. http://public.tableau.com/profile/hqi2803#!/vizhome/FallsFracturesOutcomesFramework/Landing

4. Hartholt KA, Lee R, Burns ER, Van Beeck EF. Mortality from falls among US adults aged 75 years or older, 2000–2016. JAMA. 2019; 321(21):2131–2133.

5. Agrawal Y, Merfeld D, Horak F, et al. Aging, vestibular function and balance: Proceedings of a National Institute on Aging/National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders Workshop. J. Gerontol.: Med. Sci. 2020, in press.

6. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ, Angelaki DE, et al. The Vestibular System: A Sixth Sense. New York: Oxford University Press, 2012.

7. Curthoys IS, Grant JW, Burgess AM, et al. Otolith receptor mechanisms for vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials: A review. Front. Neurol. 2018; 9:366.

8. Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang Y, et al. Anatomy and surgical approach to rat’s vestibular sensors and nerves. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2016; 270:1–8.

9. Urciuoli A, Zanolli C, Beaudet A, et al. The evolution of the vestibular apparatus in apes and humans. Elife. 2020; 9. pii:e51261.

10. Ramos de Miguel A, Zarowski A, Sluydts M, et al. The superiority of the otolith system.  Audiol Neurootol. 2020; 25(1–2):35–41.

11. Stackman RW, Clark AS, Taube JS. Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input. Hippocampus 2002; 12:291–303.

12. Russell NA, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Long-term effects of permanent vestibular lesions on hippocampal spatial firing. J. Neurosci. 2003; 23(16):6490–6498.

13. Jacob P-Y, Poucet B, Liberge M, et al. Vestibular control of entorhinal cortex activity in spatial navigation. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:38.

14. Besnard S, Lopez C, Brandt T, et al. (editors) The vestibular system in cognitive and memory processes in mammals. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. Lausanne: Frontiers Media. doi: 10.3389/978-2-88919-744-6 (ebook) pp. 1–246, 2016.

15. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2017; 30(1):84–89.

16. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971; 34(1):171–5.

17. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, et al. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2015; 436(7052):801–6.

18. Moser EI, Moser MB, McNaughton BL. Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1448–1464.

19. Smith PF. The growing evidence for the importance of the otoliths for spatial memory. Front. Neural Circ. 2019; 3(66):1–14.

20. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017; 390(10113):2673–2734.

21. Bathini P, Brai E, Auber LA. Olfactory dysfunction in the pathophysiological continuum of dementia. Ageing Res Rev. 2019; 55:100956.

22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntly J, et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. The Lancet Global Health. 2019; 7:e596–e603.

23. Michalowsky B, Hoffmann W, Kostev K. Association between hearing and vision impairment and risk of dementia: Results of a case-control study based on secondary data. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 20;11: 363.

24. Golub JS, Brickman AM, Ciarleglio AJ, et al. Association of subclinical hearing loss with cognitive performance. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2020; 146(1):57–67.

25. Loughrey DG, Parra MA, Lawlor BA. Visual short-term memory binding deficit with age-related hearing loss in cognitively normal older adults. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):12600.

26. Liu CM, Lee CT. Association of hearing loss with dementia. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(7): e198112.

27. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, et al. Longitudinal study of self-reported hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in women. Alzheimers Dement. 2020; 16(4):610–620.

28. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Rosenberg PB. Vestibular impairment, cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s Disease: Balancing the evidence. Aging and Mental Health. 2020; 24(5):705–708.

29. Smith PF, Zheng Y. From ear to uncertainty: Vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2013; 7:84.

30. Smith PF, Geddes LH, Baek J-H, et al. Modulation of cognitive function by vestibular lesions and galvanic vestibular stimulation. Front. Neurol. 2010; 1(141):1–8.

31. Smith PF, Darlington CL, Zheng Y. The effects of complete vestibular deafferentation on spatial memory and the hippocampus in rat: The Dunedin experience. In: Ferre, E.R, Harris, L. (editors), Vestibular Cognition. Brill, Leiden, 2017; pp. 461–485.

32. Baek JH, Zheng Y, Darlington C L, et al. Evidence that spatial memory deficits following bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats are probably permanent. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2010; 94(3):402–413.

33. Russell N, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2006; 96:4–14.

34. Neo P, Carter, D, Zheng, Y, et al. Septal elicitation of hippocampal theta rhythm did not repair the cognitive and emotional deficits resulting from vestibular lesions. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:1176–1187.

35. Tai SK, Ma J, Ossenkopp KP, et al. Activation of immobility-related hippocampal theta by cholinergic septohippocampal neurons during vestibular stimulation. Hippocampus. 2012; 22(4):914–25.

36. Cullen KE, Taube JS. Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges.  Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1465–1473.

37. Harvey RE, Rutan SA, Willey GR, et al. M. Linear self-motion cues support the spatial distribution and stability of hippocampal place cells. Curr. Biol. 2018; 28(11):1810.e5.

38. Aitken P, Benoit A, Zheng Y, et al. Hippocampal and striatal M1-muscarinic acetylcholine receptors are down-regulated following bilateral vestibular loss in rats. Hippocampus. 2016; 26:1509–1514.

39. Besnard S, Machado ML, Vignaux G, et al. Influence of vestibular input on spatial and nonspatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:814–826.

40. Truchet B, Benoit A, Chaillan F, et al. Hippocampal LTP modulation and glutamatergic receptors following vestibular loss. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019; 224:699–711.

41. Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:59.

42. Bohne P, Schwarz MK, Herlitze S, Mark MD. A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Front Neural Circuits. 2019; 13:51.

43. Watson TC, Obiang P, Torres-Herraez A, et al. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife. 2019; 8:e41896.

44. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Du Lac S, et al. Vestibular vertigo and comorbid cognitive and psychiatric impairment: The 2008 national health interview survey. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. 2016; 87(4):310–319.

45. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Trevino C, et al. Association between visuo- spatial ability and vestibular function in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 2015; 63(9):1837–1844.

46. Semenov YR, Bigelow RT, Xue Q, et al. Association between vestibular and cognitive function in US adults: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J. Gerontol. Series A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015; 71(2):243–250.

47. Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, et al. Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: Data from the Triangle Completion Task. Front Neurol. 2017; 8:173.

48. Wackym PA, Balaban CD, Mackay HT, et al. Longitudinal cognitive and neurobehavioral functional outcomes before and after repairing otic capsule dehiscence. Otol Neurotol. 2016; 37(1):70–82.

49. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol. 2017; 264(3):554–563.

50. Smith L, Wilkinson D, Bodani M, et al. Short-term memory impairment in vestibular patients can arise independently of psychiatric impairment, fatigue, and sleeplessness. J Neuropsychol. 2019; 13(3):417–431.

51. Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. 2005; 128(11):2732–2741.

52. Kremmyda O, Huffner K, Flanagin VL, et al. Beyond dizziness: Virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front. Human Neurosci. 2016; 10:139.

53. Ayar DA, Kumral E, Celebisoy N. Cognitive functions in acute vestibular loss. J. Neurol. 2020, in press. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w.

54. Sang FY, Jáuregui-Renaud K, Green DA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms in vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006; 77(6):760–6.

55. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang FY, Gresty MA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008; 79(3):276–83.

56. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolaños M, et al. Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res. 2008; 18(2–3):129–37.

57. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Hoffman HJ, et al. Association between vertigo, cognitive and psychiatric conditions in US children: 2012 National Health Interview Survey. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2020; 130:109802.

58. Risey J, Briner W. Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J. Vest. Res. 1990; 1:31–37.

59. Redfern MS, Talkowski ME, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive influences on postural control of patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Gait and Posture. 2004; 19:105–114.

60. Talkowski ME, Redfern MS, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive requirements for vestibular and ocular motor processing in healthy adults and patients with unilateral vestibular lesions. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2005; 17(9):1432–1441.

61. Gomez-Alvarez FB, Jáuregui-Renaud K. Psychological symptoms and spatial orientation during the first 3 months after acute vestibular neuritis. Arch. Med. Res. 2011; 42:97–103.

62. Caixeta GC dos S, Dona F, Gazzola JM. Cognitive processing and body balance in elderly subjects with vestibular dysfunction. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2012; 78(2):87–95.

63. Candidi M, Micarelli A, Viziano A, et al. Impaired mental rotation in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and acute vestibular neuritis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013; 7:783.

64. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Acute peripheral vestibular deficit increases the redundancy in random number generation. Exp. Brain Res. 2017; 235:627–637.

65. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Impaired math achievement in patients with acute vestibular neuritis. Neuropsychologia. 2017; 107:1–8.

66. Lofti Y, Rezazadeh N, Moossavi A, et al. Preliminary evidence of improved cognitive performance following vestibular rehabilitation in children with combined ADHD (cADHD) and concurrent vestibular impairment. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2017; 44:700–707.

67. Sugaya N, Arai M, Goto F. Changes in cognitive function in patients with intractable dizziness following vestibular rehabilitation. Sci. Reps. 2018; 8:9984.

68. Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, et al. Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs embodied spatial cognition. J. Neurol. 2019; 266(Suppl. 1: S149–S159.

69. Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei et al. Association between saccule and semi-circular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear. 2020; 41:686–692.

70. Dobbels B, Peetermans O, Boon B, et al. Impact of bilateral vestibulopathy on spatial and nonspatial cognition: A systematic review. Ear Hear. 2018; 40(4):757–765.

71. zu Eulenburg P, Stoeter P, Dieterich M. Voxel-based morphometry depicts central compensation after vestibular neuritis. Annal. Neurol. 2010; 68:241–249.

72. Van Cruijsen N, Hiemstra WM, Meiners LC, et al. Hippocampal volume measurement in patients with Ménière’s disease: a pilot study. Acta Otolaryngol. 2007; 127(10):1018–23.

73. Seo YJ, Kim J, Kim SH. The change of hippocampal volume and its relevance with inner ear function in Meniere’s disease patients. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2016; 43(6):620–5.

74. Göttlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, et al. Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016; 37(5):1998–2006.

75. Kamil RJ, Jacob A, Ratnanather JT, et al. Vestibular function and hippocampal volume in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA). Otol. Neurotol. 2018; (6):765–771.

76. Jacob A, Tward D, Resnick S, et al. Vestibular function and cortical and sub-cortical alterations in an aging population. Heliyon. 2020, 6: e04728.

77. Previc FH. Vestibular loss as a contributor to Alzheimer’s disease. Med. Hypoth. 2013; 80(4):360–367.

78. Chapleau M, Aldebert J, Montembeault M, Brambati SM. Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease and Semantic Dementia: An ALE Meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016; 54(3):941–955.

79. Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow RT, et al. Vestibular impairment in dementia. Otol. Neurotol. 2016; 37(8):1137–1142.

80. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Vestibular loss predicts poorer spatial cognition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017; 61(3):995–1003.

81. Kamil RJ, Bilgel M, Wong DF, et al. Vestibular function and beta-amyloid deposition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.  Front Aging Neurosci. 2018; 10:408.

82. Cui B, Zhu L, She X, et al. Chronic noise exposure causes persistence of tau hyperphosphorylation and formation of NFT tau in the rat hippocampus and pre- frontal cortex. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 238(2):122–129.

83. Mapstone M, Steffenella TM, Duffy CJ. A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment: getting lost between aging and AD. Neurol. 2003; 60(5):802–808.

84. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Increased prevalence of vestibular loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2019; 16(12):1143–1150.

85. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Saccular impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with driving difficulty. Dement. Geriatric Cog. Dis. 2017; 44(5–6):294–302.

86. Liao JY, Lee CT, Lin TY, et al. Exploring prior diseases associated with incident late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia. PLoS One. 2020; 15(1):e0228172.

87. Miyamoto T, Fukushima K, Takada T, et al. Saccular projections in the human cerebral cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2005; 1039(1):124–131.

88. Schlindwein P, Mueller M, Bauermann T, et al. Cortical representation of saccular vestibular stimulation: VEMPs in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2008; 39(1):19–31.

89. Cuthbert PC, Gilchrist DP, Hicks SL, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for vestibular activation of the guinea pig hippocampus. Neuroreport. 2000; 11(7):1443–47.

90. Smith PF, Hitier M, Zhang, Y-F, et al. Vestibular modulation of hippocampal and striatal function. J. Vest. Res. 2019; 29(1):18.

91. Le Gall A, Hilber P, Chesneau C, et al. The critical role of vestibular graviception during cognitive-motor development. Behav. Brain Res. 2019; 372:112040.

92. Cullen KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control.  Trends Neurosci. 2012; 35(3):185–96.

93. Previc F. Does vestibular dysfunction contribute to the increased prevalence of Alzheimer’s Disease in females? Clin. Case. Revs. 2019; 2(1):1–2.

94. Smith PF, Agrawal Y, Darlington CL. Sexual dimorphism in vestibular function and dysfunction. J. Neurophysiol. 2019; 121:2379–2391.

95. Hillier S, McDonnell M. Is vestibular rehabilitation effective in improving dizziness and function after unilateral peripheral vestibular hypofunction? an abridged version of a Cochrane Review. Eur. J. Phys. Rehab. Med. 2016; 52(4):541–556.

96. Herdman SJ, Clendaniel R. Vestibular Rehabilitation (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis. 2014.

97. Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech A, et al. Balance training improves memory and spatial cognition in healthy adults.  Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1):5661.

98. Gandhi P, Klatt BN, Agrawal Y. Physical and vestibular physical therapy referrals in people with Alzheimer Disease.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000390.

For the PDF of this article,
contact nzmj@nzma.org.nz

View Article PDF

Dizziness is reported to be one of the most common reasons for patients consulting a general practitioner.1 Although not all consultations regarding dizziness are related to the vestibular system, for example, they can be due to cardiovascular dysfunction, approximately 20–50% are believed to be due to balance disorders related to the peripheral vestibular system (see Table 1).1 Recent studies have estimated that 35% of US adults over the age of 35 years suffer from vestibular disorders, increasing to 85% aged 80 and over.2 Unfortunately, no reliable statistics are available on the prevalence of vestibular disorders for New Zealand. However, the Health Quality and Safety Commission reported that even between January and March, 2020, there were 65,893 fall injuries reported to the ACC, of which 8,544 were classified as serious.3 Overseas, impairment of the vestibular system has been estimated to increase the odds of falling by over 12-fold, and nearly 30% of adults aged 65 and over fall each year.2,4,5 In 2016, the mortality rate associated with falls in the US was estimated to be 122.2 per 100,000 persons.4,5

Table 1: Common causes of dizziness in primary care practice. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

The impact of vestibular dysfunction on loss of balance and falls is due both to its effects on brainstem vestibular reflex pathways as well as higher cognitive processing of self-motion signals from the vestibular system. The vestibular system (Figure 1) senses head movement (strictly speaking ‘head acceleration’ or change in head velocity) in different planes, as well as linear acceleration by gravity.6 The three semi-circular canals in each inner ear sense angular rotation of the head, and the two otoliths, the utricle and the saccule, sense linear movement.7,8 This linear movement not only includes movement of the head, forward and backward, and left and right, but linear acceleration of the head by gravity; the saccule, in particular, senses linear acceleration by gravity (Figure 2 for explanation).7 The utricle and saccule detect linear acceleration as a result of ‘otoconia’ (calcium carbonate crystals) that generate an inertial force on the hair cells during head movement, causing a change in electrical potential (see Figure 2).7 In this respect, it is important to note that the most primitive form of the otoliths (‘statoliths’) are estimated to have evolved approximately 670 million years ago, and they exist in invertebrates such as jellyfish. This is their only means of detecting upright, which is necessary for survival. Therefore, given their evolutionary age, the otoliths might be expected to have developed major contributions to balance in humans.9,10 The vestibular system, through short-latency brainstem pathways, generates rapid eye movements that compensate for the unintentional movement of the head, eg, movement of the head due to the pulse beat (the vestibulo-ocular reflexes or VORs) and maintains the stability of the visual image of the world on the retina.6 The vestibular system also generates rapid vestibulo-spinal reflexes (VSRs) which adjust posture for unintentional movement, enabling us to keep our balance.6 Without a normal vestibular system, vision would become blurred (a condition known as ‘oscillopsia’) and balance and locomotion become disrupted.6

Figure 1: Human inner ear anatomy. The cochlea and vestibular system, which encompasses the three semicircular canals (horizontal, superior (anterior vertical) and posterior vertical) and the two otolith organs (utricle and saccule), are depicted. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Figure 2: (A) Schematic representation of the plates of the otolithic receptors (the utricular and saccular maculae). The arrows show the preferred polarization of the hair cell receptors across the maculae. The dashed lines are lines of polarity reversal (lpr). The striola refers to a band of receptors on either side of the lpr. Schematics of type I (B,D) and type II receptors (C,E) show how linear acceleration acts on the otoliths and so deflects the hair bundles of individual receptors. From Curthoys et al7 with permission.

Information about angular and linear head movement is also transmitted to higher centres of the brain, where it contributes to the conscious experience of moving through the environment and to cognitive processes such as memory.11–15 As we move through the environment, the vestibular hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths detect every head movement, and transmit this information to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, where it is assimilated and stored to provide a spatial map of our movements.11–15 This information is integrated with other sensory information, such as that from the visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory and proprioceptive systems, and formulated into mathematical maps of the spatial world, allowing us to navigate through it more effectively.11–15 In the 1970s, specific neurons were discovered in the hippocampus that selectively discharged in response to specific areas of the environment. These became known as ‘place cells’.16 In the 1990s, related cells were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex, known as ‘grid cells’, which discharged in response to multiple areas in the environment.17 In 2014, the Nobel Prize in Medicine or Physiology was awarded to John O’Keefe, Edvard Moser and Britt-Mayer Moser for these discoveries, which have become known as the brain’s ‘global positioning system’.18 Since then, both place cells and grid cells have been demonstrated to rely on vestibular information from the inner ear.11–13 Therefore, one reason why vestibular-related dizziness contributes to falls, is that not only does it impair fast vestibular reflexes such as the VORs and VSRs, but it impairs the ability of the brain to integrate self-motion information and to navigate through the spatial environment and form spatial memories. Information from the vestibular system is distributed widely throughout the central nervous system and is involved in higher cognitive function.11–15,18 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths may be important for cognitive processing independently of the semi-circular canals;19 this is one reason why the evolutionary age of the otoliths is of interest.

In recent years, a substantial amount of epidemiological evidence has been published to support the idea that age-related hearing loss is a risk factor for dementia.20 For example, in a seminal study published in the Lancet, it was reported that the contribution of hearing loss to the incidence of dementia was greater than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity and social isolation.20 This result seemed surprising, because sensory systems had never been considered particularly important to dementia, except perhaps for olfactory function as a potential biomarker.21 It is important to note that the Livingstone et al study20 was based on data from high-income countries and the evidence from low-to-middle income countries is less convincing in this respect.22 Over the last several years, further evidence in support of the importance of hearing loss for the development of dementia has been published, although it is not entirely consistent.22–27

Less attention has been given to the other part of the inner ear, the vestibular system (see Figure 1); however, evidence is mounting that age-related vestibular disorders could also be a significant risk factor for the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia, along with hearing loss.28 The vestibular system is known to degenerate with age, as with other sensory systems, with decreases in hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths, a reduction in the number of neurons in the vestibular nerve and brainstem vestibular nucleus, and a deterioration of vestibular reflex responses.1,2 As shown in the Iceberg model in Figure 3, clinical presentation of age-related vestibular symptoms usually includes ‘presbystasis’ (the imbalance of disequilibrium) or ‘presbyvertigo’ (vertigo), or both, possibly with a decrease in vestibular perception; however, it is possible for age-related vestibular symptoms to be sub-clinical and therefore harder to detect .

Figure 3: Iceberg model of age-related vestibular loss.

VSR: vestibulo-spinal reflex; VOR: vestibulo-ocular reflex. ‘Presbystasis’: age-related imbalance of disequilibrium. ‘Presbyvertigo’: age-related vertigo. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Animal studies supporting the role of the vestibular system in cognitive function

The literature relating the vestibular system to cognitive function, especially spatial memory, dates back to the 1960’s.14 Numerous studies have reported evidence that unilateral or bilateral lesions of the peripheral vestibular system impair spatial memory in various maze and foraging tasks.14,29–31 In some cases, these have been conducted even 14 months after bilateral vestibular lesions (BVL) in rats, and the spatial memory deficits remain.32 A variety of potentially confounding factors have been controlled for, including vision, degree of motor activity, anxiety and auditory function, and the results have been consistent.29–32 BVL has been demonstrated to impair the function of neurons in the hippocampus that encode places in the environment (‘hippocampal place cells’),11,12 EEG activity in the theta frequency range,33–35 which is thought to regulate place cell function, and theta EEG activity among grid cells of the entorhinal cortex.13 Neurons in the thalamus which encode head direction (‘head direction cells’), are also dysfunctional following BVL.36 Together, these abnormalities in the function of place cells, grid cells and head direction cells, are likely to underlie the spatial memory deficits observed in animals.14,15 Furthermore, transgenic mice without otoconia and therefore without otolith function (‘otolith deficient tilted mice’), but with normal semi-circular canal function, have been shown to have aberrant hippocampal place cell and head direction cell activity.36,37 In addition, a variety of neurochemical changes have been documented in the hippocampus following BVL, including changes in the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype of glutamate receptor and muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptors,38–40 both of which are implicated in hippocampal learning and memory processes.

Taken together, the data from animal studies strongly indicate an important role for the vestibular system in spatial cognition. Although the specific pathways through which vestibular information reaches areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, are yet to be fully elucidated, projections from the vestibular nucleus and cerebellum are likely to transmit information via multiple routes, particularly the thalamus (see Figure 4).41 It is speculated that there is a ‘theta pathway’, which involves projections from the brainstem vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) to the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg), then via various nuclei to the medial septum, which releases ACh into the hippocampus; a ‘head direction pathway’, from the VNC via head direction cells of the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (ADN) to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), to the hippocampus; a major thalamic pathway which transmits vestibular information to the parietal cortex and then on to the MEC and hippocampus; and a transcerebellar pathway.41–43 The detailed pathways are depicted in Figure 4.

Figure 4: ADN, anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; Interpositus N, anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei; LMN, lateral mammillary nuclei; MEC, medial entorhinal cortex; MG, medial geniculate nucleus; NPH, nucleus prepositus hypoglossi; Parietal C, Parietal cortex; PaS, parasubiculum; Perirhinal, Perirhinal cortex; PoS, posterior subiculum (i.e dorsal part of the presubiculum); Post HT, posterior hypothalamus; Postrhinal, postrhinal cortex; PPTg , pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Pulv, pulvinar; RPO, reticularis pontis oralis; SUM, supramammillary nucleus; ViM, ventralis intermedius nuclei of the thalamus; VLN, ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VNC, vestibular nucleus complex; VPi, ventral posterior inferior nucleus of the thalamus; VPL, ventral posterior lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPM, ventral posterior medial nuclei of the thalamus. From Hitier et al.41 with permission.

Vestibular contributions to cognitive function in humans

Consistent with the studies in animals, many studies conducted over the last two decades have demonstrated that vestibular disorders are associated with the impairment of cognitive function in otherwise normal healthy adults.44–53 The studies reviewed were identified using a Pubmed search between 1989 and 2020, using ‘vestibular’ and ‘cognition’ as key words. All of the studies were included; they consisted of two main types: survey and epidemiological studies; and clinical experimental studies (Table 2). Among the symptoms that have been reported are difficulty concentrating, deficits in attention and spatial memory, verbal fluency, mental rotation, and dyscalculia and other forms of numerical cognition (see Table 2). Although some 51 such studies have been published since 1989, only a subset of them (21) have controlled for hearing loss, either by excluding patients with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in the analysis of the data.54–69 Table 2 provides a summary of the studies that have controlled for hearing loss and therefore where the deficits can be considered to be mainly vestibular in origin. Dobbels et al70 have argued that few of the studies reporting cognitive deficits in humans with vestibular disorders, have controlled for hearing loss. However, a careful review of the literature suggests that is not the case. Of course, there are cases in which vestibular and auditory symptoms present together, for example, in Meniere’s disease, in which case they are both likely to contribute to cognitive deficits. Beyond controlling for hearing loss, the major weaknesses of the epidemiological studies are that they often include ‘heterogeneous vestibular disorders’ (bilateral vestibular loss, unilateral vestibular loss, vestibular neuritis, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), etc.) and do not include the same controls as the experimental studies (see Table 2). The available experimental studies are based on samples of patients with different vestibular disorders, but each one of them includes a sample of patients which is compared to a control group without vestibular dysfunction (see Table 2).

Table 2: Studies conducted in humans that have reported cognitive deficits associated with different types of vestibular dysfunction, in which hearing loss has been controlled for in some way, either by excluding subjects with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in a multiple logistic regression model. Where ‘No ()’ occurs, the first number in the brackets indicates the number of subjects without hearing loss and the second, the total sample size.

Abbreviations: UVL, unilateral vestibular loss; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; UVN, unilateral vestibular neurectomy; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; RT, reaction time.

Studies in humans which have combined structural and functional neuro-imaging with behavioural assays, have also provided compelling evidence that vestibular sensory inputs are important for human spatial cognition. In a seminal study of patients with neurofibromatosis type 2 (NF2) who underwent bilateral vestibular nerve section, it was observed that the NF2 patients exhibited significantly poorer spatial navigation skills, measured using a virtual Morris Water Maze Task, which required no movement other than that of a mouse to control a cursor on a computer screen. These spatial cognitive deficits were correlated with reduced hippocampal volumes (approximately 17%) compared to age- and sex-matched controls.51 Only one of these patients exhibited total hearing loss post-operatively and all of them were 8–10 years post-BVL. Subsequent studies have provided further evidence of impaired spatial memory and hippocampal atrophy in patients with other vestibular disorders such as Meniere’s disease.52,71–74 A recent study of over 100 healthy adults reported that poorer vestibular function was correlated with significantly reduced hippocampal volume.75 In our most recent study, we have found that age is statistically related to a bilateral decrease in the volume of the hippocampus and the left entorhinal cortex.76

Vestibular dysfunction as a risk factor for dementia

As a result of the animal and human studies demonstrating that peripheral vestibular lesions caused spatial memory deficits, Previc77 suggested that loss of vestibular function might be implicated in the development of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). His argument was based partly on the limitations of the β-amyloid (Aβ) hypothesis of AD, but the idea can be considered independently of that hypothesis. The central vestibular system, including the brainstem VNC, contributes to major cholinergic inputs to the hippocampus, which is damaged in AD.78 Bilateral vestibular loss in rats also results in a decrease in acetylcholine (ACh) receptors in the hippocampus.38

There have been only a few studies that have directly investigated the relationship between vestibular function and AD, all of them conducted by the same group. The first studies investigated the statistical relationship between vestibular dysfunction and clinical syndromes of cognitive impairment, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. Harun et al79 investigated the prevalence of vestibular dysfunction in 32 patients with AD and 15 with MCI, compared to 94 controls, and estimated that patients with bilaterally absent cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMPs, a measure of saccular function), had a greater than three-fold increased odds of AD (OR 3.42, 95% CI 1.32–8.91, P=0.011). Furthermore, a 1 µV increase in cVEMP amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.28, 95% CI 0.09–0.93, P=0.038). Higher ocular VEMP (oVEMP, indicative of utricular function) amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.92, 95% CI 0.85–0.99, P=0.036). However, there was no significant difference between the MCI group and the controls. Importantly, there was no significant association between VOR function and AD, indicating that semi-circular canal function was not implicated, only the otoliths, the most primitive part of the vestibular system.

In a follow-up study, Wei et al80 examined vestibular function in 51 patients with AD, 26 with MCI and 295 matched controls. The cVEMP, the oVEMP and the VOR were all tested. Compared to controls, they found that people with cVEMP impairment had a 3–4 fold increase in the odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.0, 95% CI 1.1–8.5, P=0.04) and those with oVEMP impairment had an almost four-fold increased odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.9, 95% CI 1.4–11.3, P=0.01). Compared to controls, they found that people with impaired cVEMPs had a five-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 5.0, 95% CI 2.0–12.3, P=0.001) and those with abnormal oVEMPs had a greater than four-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 4.2, 95% CI 1.9–9.1, P≤0.001). Importantly, VOR gain (the ratio of head velocity to eye velocity) was not significantly related to group membership.

There are a number of limitations of these studies which must be noted, however. First, hearing loss was not controlled for in these studies of AD and MCI, so it is possible that it may have been a contributing factor. Second, the sample sizes of AD patients were relatively small, n=32 and 51, and therefore the studies need to be replicated.79,80 Third, the studies were cross-sectional, and the samples may not have been representative of a broader population.80 In particular, patients with both vestibular and cognitive impairment may have been more likely to present than those with either condition alone, resulting in a potential overestimation of the proportion of vestibular dysfunction in AD (‘Berksonian bias’).80 Fourth, it is conceivable that the poor performance by AD patients on the cVEMP and oVEMP testing could have been due to their inability to understand and follow instructions; however, the authors reported that this was not the case.79,80 Finally, the relationship between cVEMP/oVEMP function and AD was a statistical one involving logistic regression and does not necessarily indicate a causal relationship.79,80 For example, aside from the possibility that vestibular dysfunction contributed to the development of AD, it is possible that AD pathology might have caused vestibular dysfunction.

One potential explanation for the relationship between vestibular dysfunction and AD might be that AD pathology (eg, β-amyloid (Aβ)) extends into the central vestibular pathways from the vestibular nucleus to the thalamus and beyond, thereby impairing vestibular function. Although there have been no specific studies of Aβ deposition in ‘vestibular-related areas’ of the brain, vestibular information is distributed widely,41 (see Figure 4), therefore it is likely that AD pathology extends to many brain regions receiving vestibular input. However, in a recent study of vestibular function in 98 participants aged 77.3 (±8.26) from the BLSA, Aβ deposition was measured using amyloid C-11 Pittsburgh Compound B (11C-PB).81 The authors found that 22.4% of the sample were positive for PiB; however, there was no statistically significant relationship between the extent of Aβ deposition and any measure of vestibular function. This study was designed to investigate preclinical AD, but no such study has been performed in patients diagnosed with AD. Another possible explanation is that vestibular impairment directly contributes to medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration and AD, possibly as a result of reduced vestibular sensory input to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, as occurs for auditory input.82

Further studies have concentrated on whether vestibular loss is associated with specific phenotypes of AD, especially those with spatial cognitive deficits. Some AD phenotypes are characterised by predominantly amnestic symptoms compared to others which are characterised by more motoric and spatial impairment.83 In a study of 50 patients with MCI or AD, Wei et al84 observed that patients with vestibular loss were significantly more likely to exhibit impairment in neurocognitive tests of spatial skills, for example the Money Road Map test (MRMT). When patients were divided into ‘spatially normal’ and ‘spatially impaired’ groups based on their performance in the MRMT, only 25% of the spatially normal patients were found to have vestibular dysfunction compared to 96% of the spatially impaired patients.84 In a further study of 60 patients with MCI or AD, patients with vestibular dysfunction were significantly more likely to have difficulty driving, an activity closely linked to spatial cognitive ability.85 It is possible, therefore, that vestibular dysfunction contributes to the development of a ‘spatial’ subtype of AD, increasing the probability of symptoms such as spatial disorientation, wandering, and an increased risk of falling.84 However, these two studies have similar limitations to the original one by Harun et al:79 there were no controls for hearing loss, the sample sizes were relatively small, the studies were cross-sectional and in Wei et al,85 the postural measurements were not specific to vestibular function. Finally, the statistical association reported does not indicate causality.

The only available study that provides any evidence that vestibular loss might be causally involved in the development of cognitive impairment and dementia is by Liao et al86 They investigated prior medical conditions that were associated with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia (LOAD) using a population-based matched case control study based on the National Health Insurance Research database of Taiwan and the Catastrophic Illness Certificate database, between the years 1997 and 2013. The definitions of prior diseases were based on the first three digits of the International Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM). The total case group consisted of 4,600 patients who were newly diagnosed with LOAD between 2007 and 2013, who were then matched to 4,600 controls by both age and sex. Using multivariate logistic regression and path analysis, the authors reported that the incidence of LOAD was positively correlated with prior anxiety (ICD code 300), functional digestive disorder (ICD code 564), psychopathology-specific symptoms (ICD code 307), disorders of the vestibular system (ICD code 386), concussion (ICD code 850), disorders of the urethra and urinary tract (ICD code 599), disorders of refraction and accommodation (ICD code 367) and hearing loss (ICD code 389). While the authors conclude that these data suggest that vestibular dysfunction may therefore be a risk factor for LOAD, the limitations of the study include limited information regarding other confounding factors such as body mass index, blood pressure, diet, smoking, diabetic therapy etc.; and the specific nature of the diagnosis may have varied according to factors affecting access to a neurologist. At present, there are no comparable data available on potential vestibular contributions to dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease or fronto-temporal dementia.

One of the intriguing aspects of the studies in cognitively-normal and vestibular-impaired adults is the demonstration of a link between saccular function and cognition.45,47,69,75,76,79,80,85 We have recently reported that saccular function is a statistically significant predictor of the decrease in hippocampal volume that occurs with age.76 The saccule, one of the two otoliths, is the part of the peripheral vestibular system which detects the orientation of the head with respect to gravity and, together with the utricle, is the oldest component of the vestibular system in evolutionary terms (see Figure 2). Patients with AD exhibited specifically poorer saccular and utricular function compared to age-matched controls.79,85 Saccular stimulation has been demonstrated to activate the multisensory vestibular cortex involved in spatial information processing.87,88 In guinea pigs and rats, selective electrical stimulation of the utricle and saccule has been shown to cause widespread activation of the hippocampus.89,90 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths, the saccule in particular, have a critical role in spatial memory due to their importance in the perception of gravitational vertical.19 We have recently reported that mice lacking otolithic function from birth, exhibit major developmental delays by post-natal day 9, including spatial memory deficits.91 Interestingly, considerable electrophysiological evidence suggests that the neurons in the VNC that subserve the VOR are separate from those involved in the VSR pathways and the pathways to the limbic system and neocortex.92 This means that it is possible for the vestibular pathways that give rise to the conscious perception of self-movement and contribute to spatial memory, to be compromised, without VOR deficits necessarily being exhibited, and that only VSRs such as VEMPs would indicate a vestibular deficit. Figure 5 summarises the hypothesis that saccular dysfunction, in particular, might contribute to the development of cognitive dysfunction that preferentially includes spatial cognitive deficits.

Figure 5: Conceptual model of impact of aging on vestibular function (notably saccular function), which contributes to neurodegeneration of neural circuits involved in vestibular processing and deterioration, specifically in spatial cognitive ability. From Agrawal et al28 with permission.

Conclusions

The available evidence suggests that vestibular dysfunction, including that associated with age-related vestibular loss, has a significant negative impact on cognitive function. Vestibular impairment may therefore be a risk factor for the development of dementia, including AD. Previc93 has recently suggested that, given the increased prevalence of vestibular disorders in females,94 vestibular dysfunction may contribute to their increased incidence of AD. However, the majority of the evidence to date is correlational; therefore, caution must be exercised in interpreting these findings. The potential combined effects of both hearing loss and vestibular loss are unknown but could be expected to be much greater. Of course, it is important to note that some otological disorders involve both auditory and vestibular symptoms (eg, Meniere’s disease). Understanding the full implications of vestibular dysfunction for cognitive decline is potentially of great importance for the health of the elderly, since effective therapies are available to treat vestibular disorders.95 One of the principal treatments for vestibular impairment is vestibular rehabilitation, a suite of physical therapy-based exercises in which head movements are used to stimulate the vestibular system and gradually encourage the brain to adapt to the loss of normal vestibular function.96 Several studies have reported that vestibular rehabilitation can improve cognitive function in healthy adults and in patients with intractable dizziness.67,97 However, at present, studies from the US indicate that only a small number of people with AD are referred for vestibular rehabilitation.98 Since vestibular impairment may be a modifiable risk factor for dementia, the impact of vestibular loss on cognition should be considered along with hearing loss as a critical area for research.

Summary

Abstract

Dementia is recognised to be one of the most challenging diseases facing society, both now and in the future, with its prevalence estimated to increase substantially by 2050. The potential contributions of age-related sensory deficits have attracted little attention until recently, when a landmark study suggested that hearing loss could be a greater risk factor for dementia than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity or social isolation. Over the last decade, evidence has been gradually accumulating to suggest that the other part of the inner ear, the balance organs or ‘vestibular system’, might also be important in the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Increasing evidence suggests that dizziness associated with vestibular dysfunction, a common reason for patients consulting their GPs, increases the risk of cognitive dysfunction, including dementia, and our understanding of the basic neurobiology of this sensory system supports this view. This paper aims to review and critically evaluate the relevant evidence.

Aim

Method

Results

Conclusion

Author Information

Paul F Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the three anonymous referees for their constructive and helpful comments on the manuscript.

Correspondence

Prof Paul Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Correspondence Email

paul.smith@otago.ac.nz

Competing Interests

Nil.

1. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Merfeld D. Dizziness, imbalance and age-related vestibular loss. In: Straka H, editor, Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier, NY, 2020.

2. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, et al. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the national health and nutrition examination survey, 2001-2004. Arch. Intern. Med. 2009; 169(10):938–944.

3. Health Quality and Safety Commission New Zealand, 2020. http://public.tableau.com/profile/hqi2803#!/vizhome/FallsFracturesOutcomesFramework/Landing

4. Hartholt KA, Lee R, Burns ER, Van Beeck EF. Mortality from falls among US adults aged 75 years or older, 2000–2016. JAMA. 2019; 321(21):2131–2133.

5. Agrawal Y, Merfeld D, Horak F, et al. Aging, vestibular function and balance: Proceedings of a National Institute on Aging/National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders Workshop. J. Gerontol.: Med. Sci. 2020, in press.

6. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ, Angelaki DE, et al. The Vestibular System: A Sixth Sense. New York: Oxford University Press, 2012.

7. Curthoys IS, Grant JW, Burgess AM, et al. Otolith receptor mechanisms for vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials: A review. Front. Neurol. 2018; 9:366.

8. Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang Y, et al. Anatomy and surgical approach to rat’s vestibular sensors and nerves. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2016; 270:1–8.

9. Urciuoli A, Zanolli C, Beaudet A, et al. The evolution of the vestibular apparatus in apes and humans. Elife. 2020; 9. pii:e51261.

10. Ramos de Miguel A, Zarowski A, Sluydts M, et al. The superiority of the otolith system.  Audiol Neurootol. 2020; 25(1–2):35–41.

11. Stackman RW, Clark AS, Taube JS. Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input. Hippocampus 2002; 12:291–303.

12. Russell NA, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Long-term effects of permanent vestibular lesions on hippocampal spatial firing. J. Neurosci. 2003; 23(16):6490–6498.

13. Jacob P-Y, Poucet B, Liberge M, et al. Vestibular control of entorhinal cortex activity in spatial navigation. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:38.

14. Besnard S, Lopez C, Brandt T, et al. (editors) The vestibular system in cognitive and memory processes in mammals. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. Lausanne: Frontiers Media. doi: 10.3389/978-2-88919-744-6 (ebook) pp. 1–246, 2016.

15. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2017; 30(1):84–89.

16. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971; 34(1):171–5.

17. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, et al. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2015; 436(7052):801–6.

18. Moser EI, Moser MB, McNaughton BL. Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1448–1464.

19. Smith PF. The growing evidence for the importance of the otoliths for spatial memory. Front. Neural Circ. 2019; 3(66):1–14.

20. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017; 390(10113):2673–2734.

21. Bathini P, Brai E, Auber LA. Olfactory dysfunction in the pathophysiological continuum of dementia. Ageing Res Rev. 2019; 55:100956.

22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntly J, et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. The Lancet Global Health. 2019; 7:e596–e603.

23. Michalowsky B, Hoffmann W, Kostev K. Association between hearing and vision impairment and risk of dementia: Results of a case-control study based on secondary data. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 20;11: 363.

24. Golub JS, Brickman AM, Ciarleglio AJ, et al. Association of subclinical hearing loss with cognitive performance. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2020; 146(1):57–67.

25. Loughrey DG, Parra MA, Lawlor BA. Visual short-term memory binding deficit with age-related hearing loss in cognitively normal older adults. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):12600.

26. Liu CM, Lee CT. Association of hearing loss with dementia. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(7): e198112.

27. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, et al. Longitudinal study of self-reported hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in women. Alzheimers Dement. 2020; 16(4):610–620.

28. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Rosenberg PB. Vestibular impairment, cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s Disease: Balancing the evidence. Aging and Mental Health. 2020; 24(5):705–708.

29. Smith PF, Zheng Y. From ear to uncertainty: Vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2013; 7:84.

30. Smith PF, Geddes LH, Baek J-H, et al. Modulation of cognitive function by vestibular lesions and galvanic vestibular stimulation. Front. Neurol. 2010; 1(141):1–8.

31. Smith PF, Darlington CL, Zheng Y. The effects of complete vestibular deafferentation on spatial memory and the hippocampus in rat: The Dunedin experience. In: Ferre, E.R, Harris, L. (editors), Vestibular Cognition. Brill, Leiden, 2017; pp. 461–485.

32. Baek JH, Zheng Y, Darlington C L, et al. Evidence that spatial memory deficits following bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats are probably permanent. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2010; 94(3):402–413.

33. Russell N, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2006; 96:4–14.

34. Neo P, Carter, D, Zheng, Y, et al. Septal elicitation of hippocampal theta rhythm did not repair the cognitive and emotional deficits resulting from vestibular lesions. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:1176–1187.

35. Tai SK, Ma J, Ossenkopp KP, et al. Activation of immobility-related hippocampal theta by cholinergic septohippocampal neurons during vestibular stimulation. Hippocampus. 2012; 22(4):914–25.

36. Cullen KE, Taube JS. Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges.  Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1465–1473.

37. Harvey RE, Rutan SA, Willey GR, et al. M. Linear self-motion cues support the spatial distribution and stability of hippocampal place cells. Curr. Biol. 2018; 28(11):1810.e5.

38. Aitken P, Benoit A, Zheng Y, et al. Hippocampal and striatal M1-muscarinic acetylcholine receptors are down-regulated following bilateral vestibular loss in rats. Hippocampus. 2016; 26:1509–1514.

39. Besnard S, Machado ML, Vignaux G, et al. Influence of vestibular input on spatial and nonspatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:814–826.

40. Truchet B, Benoit A, Chaillan F, et al. Hippocampal LTP modulation and glutamatergic receptors following vestibular loss. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019; 224:699–711.

41. Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:59.

42. Bohne P, Schwarz MK, Herlitze S, Mark MD. A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Front Neural Circuits. 2019; 13:51.

43. Watson TC, Obiang P, Torres-Herraez A, et al. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife. 2019; 8:e41896.

44. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Du Lac S, et al. Vestibular vertigo and comorbid cognitive and psychiatric impairment: The 2008 national health interview survey. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. 2016; 87(4):310–319.

45. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Trevino C, et al. Association between visuo- spatial ability and vestibular function in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 2015; 63(9):1837–1844.

46. Semenov YR, Bigelow RT, Xue Q, et al. Association between vestibular and cognitive function in US adults: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J. Gerontol. Series A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015; 71(2):243–250.

47. Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, et al. Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: Data from the Triangle Completion Task. Front Neurol. 2017; 8:173.

48. Wackym PA, Balaban CD, Mackay HT, et al. Longitudinal cognitive and neurobehavioral functional outcomes before and after repairing otic capsule dehiscence. Otol Neurotol. 2016; 37(1):70–82.

49. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol. 2017; 264(3):554–563.

50. Smith L, Wilkinson D, Bodani M, et al. Short-term memory impairment in vestibular patients can arise independently of psychiatric impairment, fatigue, and sleeplessness. J Neuropsychol. 2019; 13(3):417–431.

51. Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. 2005; 128(11):2732–2741.

52. Kremmyda O, Huffner K, Flanagin VL, et al. Beyond dizziness: Virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front. Human Neurosci. 2016; 10:139.

53. Ayar DA, Kumral E, Celebisoy N. Cognitive functions in acute vestibular loss. J. Neurol. 2020, in press. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w.

54. Sang FY, Jáuregui-Renaud K, Green DA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms in vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006; 77(6):760–6.

55. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang FY, Gresty MA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008; 79(3):276–83.

56. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolaños M, et al. Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res. 2008; 18(2–3):129–37.

57. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Hoffman HJ, et al. Association between vertigo, cognitive and psychiatric conditions in US children: 2012 National Health Interview Survey. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2020; 130:109802.

58. Risey J, Briner W. Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J. Vest. Res. 1990; 1:31–37.

59. Redfern MS, Talkowski ME, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive influences on postural control of patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Gait and Posture. 2004; 19:105–114.

60. Talkowski ME, Redfern MS, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive requirements for vestibular and ocular motor processing in healthy adults and patients with unilateral vestibular lesions. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2005; 17(9):1432–1441.

61. Gomez-Alvarez FB, Jáuregui-Renaud K. Psychological symptoms and spatial orientation during the first 3 months after acute vestibular neuritis. Arch. Med. Res. 2011; 42:97–103.

62. Caixeta GC dos S, Dona F, Gazzola JM. Cognitive processing and body balance in elderly subjects with vestibular dysfunction. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2012; 78(2):87–95.

63. Candidi M, Micarelli A, Viziano A, et al. Impaired mental rotation in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and acute vestibular neuritis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013; 7:783.

64. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Acute peripheral vestibular deficit increases the redundancy in random number generation. Exp. Brain Res. 2017; 235:627–637.

65. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Impaired math achievement in patients with acute vestibular neuritis. Neuropsychologia. 2017; 107:1–8.

66. Lofti Y, Rezazadeh N, Moossavi A, et al. Preliminary evidence of improved cognitive performance following vestibular rehabilitation in children with combined ADHD (cADHD) and concurrent vestibular impairment. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2017; 44:700–707.

67. Sugaya N, Arai M, Goto F. Changes in cognitive function in patients with intractable dizziness following vestibular rehabilitation. Sci. Reps. 2018; 8:9984.

68. Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, et al. Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs embodied spatial cognition. J. Neurol. 2019; 266(Suppl. 1: S149–S159.

69. Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei et al. Association between saccule and semi-circular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear. 2020; 41:686–692.

70. Dobbels B, Peetermans O, Boon B, et al. Impact of bilateral vestibulopathy on spatial and nonspatial cognition: A systematic review. Ear Hear. 2018; 40(4):757–765.

71. zu Eulenburg P, Stoeter P, Dieterich M. Voxel-based morphometry depicts central compensation after vestibular neuritis. Annal. Neurol. 2010; 68:241–249.

72. Van Cruijsen N, Hiemstra WM, Meiners LC, et al. Hippocampal volume measurement in patients with Ménière’s disease: a pilot study. Acta Otolaryngol. 2007; 127(10):1018–23.

73. Seo YJ, Kim J, Kim SH. The change of hippocampal volume and its relevance with inner ear function in Meniere’s disease patients. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2016; 43(6):620–5.

74. Göttlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, et al. Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016; 37(5):1998–2006.

75. Kamil RJ, Jacob A, Ratnanather JT, et al. Vestibular function and hippocampal volume in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA). Otol. Neurotol. 2018; (6):765–771.

76. Jacob A, Tward D, Resnick S, et al. Vestibular function and cortical and sub-cortical alterations in an aging population. Heliyon. 2020, 6: e04728.

77. Previc FH. Vestibular loss as a contributor to Alzheimer’s disease. Med. Hypoth. 2013; 80(4):360–367.

78. Chapleau M, Aldebert J, Montembeault M, Brambati SM. Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease and Semantic Dementia: An ALE Meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016; 54(3):941–955.

79. Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow RT, et al. Vestibular impairment in dementia. Otol. Neurotol. 2016; 37(8):1137–1142.

80. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Vestibular loss predicts poorer spatial cognition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017; 61(3):995–1003.

81. Kamil RJ, Bilgel M, Wong DF, et al. Vestibular function and beta-amyloid deposition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.  Front Aging Neurosci. 2018; 10:408.

82. Cui B, Zhu L, She X, et al. Chronic noise exposure causes persistence of tau hyperphosphorylation and formation of NFT tau in the rat hippocampus and pre- frontal cortex. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 238(2):122–129.

83. Mapstone M, Steffenella TM, Duffy CJ. A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment: getting lost between aging and AD. Neurol. 2003; 60(5):802–808.

84. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Increased prevalence of vestibular loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2019; 16(12):1143–1150.

85. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Saccular impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with driving difficulty. Dement. Geriatric Cog. Dis. 2017; 44(5–6):294–302.

86. Liao JY, Lee CT, Lin TY, et al. Exploring prior diseases associated with incident late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia. PLoS One. 2020; 15(1):e0228172.

87. Miyamoto T, Fukushima K, Takada T, et al. Saccular projections in the human cerebral cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2005; 1039(1):124–131.

88. Schlindwein P, Mueller M, Bauermann T, et al. Cortical representation of saccular vestibular stimulation: VEMPs in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2008; 39(1):19–31.

89. Cuthbert PC, Gilchrist DP, Hicks SL, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for vestibular activation of the guinea pig hippocampus. Neuroreport. 2000; 11(7):1443–47.

90. Smith PF, Hitier M, Zhang, Y-F, et al. Vestibular modulation of hippocampal and striatal function. J. Vest. Res. 2019; 29(1):18.

91. Le Gall A, Hilber P, Chesneau C, et al. The critical role of vestibular graviception during cognitive-motor development. Behav. Brain Res. 2019; 372:112040.

92. Cullen KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control.  Trends Neurosci. 2012; 35(3):185–96.

93. Previc F. Does vestibular dysfunction contribute to the increased prevalence of Alzheimer’s Disease in females? Clin. Case. Revs. 2019; 2(1):1–2.

94. Smith PF, Agrawal Y, Darlington CL. Sexual dimorphism in vestibular function and dysfunction. J. Neurophysiol. 2019; 121:2379–2391.

95. Hillier S, McDonnell M. Is vestibular rehabilitation effective in improving dizziness and function after unilateral peripheral vestibular hypofunction? an abridged version of a Cochrane Review. Eur. J. Phys. Rehab. Med. 2016; 52(4):541–556.

96. Herdman SJ, Clendaniel R. Vestibular Rehabilitation (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis. 2014.

97. Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech A, et al. Balance training improves memory and spatial cognition in healthy adults.  Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1):5661.

98. Gandhi P, Klatt BN, Agrawal Y. Physical and vestibular physical therapy referrals in people with Alzheimer Disease.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000390.

Contact diana@nzma.org.nz
for the PDF of this article

View Article PDF

Dizziness is reported to be one of the most common reasons for patients consulting a general practitioner.1 Although not all consultations regarding dizziness are related to the vestibular system, for example, they can be due to cardiovascular dysfunction, approximately 20–50% are believed to be due to balance disorders related to the peripheral vestibular system (see Table 1).1 Recent studies have estimated that 35% of US adults over the age of 35 years suffer from vestibular disorders, increasing to 85% aged 80 and over.2 Unfortunately, no reliable statistics are available on the prevalence of vestibular disorders for New Zealand. However, the Health Quality and Safety Commission reported that even between January and March, 2020, there were 65,893 fall injuries reported to the ACC, of which 8,544 were classified as serious.3 Overseas, impairment of the vestibular system has been estimated to increase the odds of falling by over 12-fold, and nearly 30% of adults aged 65 and over fall each year.2,4,5 In 2016, the mortality rate associated with falls in the US was estimated to be 122.2 per 100,000 persons.4,5

Table 1: Common causes of dizziness in primary care practice. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

The impact of vestibular dysfunction on loss of balance and falls is due both to its effects on brainstem vestibular reflex pathways as well as higher cognitive processing of self-motion signals from the vestibular system. The vestibular system (Figure 1) senses head movement (strictly speaking ‘head acceleration’ or change in head velocity) in different planes, as well as linear acceleration by gravity.6 The three semi-circular canals in each inner ear sense angular rotation of the head, and the two otoliths, the utricle and the saccule, sense linear movement.7,8 This linear movement not only includes movement of the head, forward and backward, and left and right, but linear acceleration of the head by gravity; the saccule, in particular, senses linear acceleration by gravity (Figure 2 for explanation).7 The utricle and saccule detect linear acceleration as a result of ‘otoconia’ (calcium carbonate crystals) that generate an inertial force on the hair cells during head movement, causing a change in electrical potential (see Figure 2).7 In this respect, it is important to note that the most primitive form of the otoliths (‘statoliths’) are estimated to have evolved approximately 670 million years ago, and they exist in invertebrates such as jellyfish. This is their only means of detecting upright, which is necessary for survival. Therefore, given their evolutionary age, the otoliths might be expected to have developed major contributions to balance in humans.9,10 The vestibular system, through short-latency brainstem pathways, generates rapid eye movements that compensate for the unintentional movement of the head, eg, movement of the head due to the pulse beat (the vestibulo-ocular reflexes or VORs) and maintains the stability of the visual image of the world on the retina.6 The vestibular system also generates rapid vestibulo-spinal reflexes (VSRs) which adjust posture for unintentional movement, enabling us to keep our balance.6 Without a normal vestibular system, vision would become blurred (a condition known as ‘oscillopsia’) and balance and locomotion become disrupted.6

Figure 1: Human inner ear anatomy. The cochlea and vestibular system, which encompasses the three semicircular canals (horizontal, superior (anterior vertical) and posterior vertical) and the two otolith organs (utricle and saccule), are depicted. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Figure 2: (A) Schematic representation of the plates of the otolithic receptors (the utricular and saccular maculae). The arrows show the preferred polarization of the hair cell receptors across the maculae. The dashed lines are lines of polarity reversal (lpr). The striola refers to a band of receptors on either side of the lpr. Schematics of type I (B,D) and type II receptors (C,E) show how linear acceleration acts on the otoliths and so deflects the hair bundles of individual receptors. From Curthoys et al7 with permission.

Information about angular and linear head movement is also transmitted to higher centres of the brain, where it contributes to the conscious experience of moving through the environment and to cognitive processes such as memory.11–15 As we move through the environment, the vestibular hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths detect every head movement, and transmit this information to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, where it is assimilated and stored to provide a spatial map of our movements.11–15 This information is integrated with other sensory information, such as that from the visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory and proprioceptive systems, and formulated into mathematical maps of the spatial world, allowing us to navigate through it more effectively.11–15 In the 1970s, specific neurons were discovered in the hippocampus that selectively discharged in response to specific areas of the environment. These became known as ‘place cells’.16 In the 1990s, related cells were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex, known as ‘grid cells’, which discharged in response to multiple areas in the environment.17 In 2014, the Nobel Prize in Medicine or Physiology was awarded to John O’Keefe, Edvard Moser and Britt-Mayer Moser for these discoveries, which have become known as the brain’s ‘global positioning system’.18 Since then, both place cells and grid cells have been demonstrated to rely on vestibular information from the inner ear.11–13 Therefore, one reason why vestibular-related dizziness contributes to falls, is that not only does it impair fast vestibular reflexes such as the VORs and VSRs, but it impairs the ability of the brain to integrate self-motion information and to navigate through the spatial environment and form spatial memories. Information from the vestibular system is distributed widely throughout the central nervous system and is involved in higher cognitive function.11–15,18 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths may be important for cognitive processing independently of the semi-circular canals;19 this is one reason why the evolutionary age of the otoliths is of interest.

In recent years, a substantial amount of epidemiological evidence has been published to support the idea that age-related hearing loss is a risk factor for dementia.20 For example, in a seminal study published in the Lancet, it was reported that the contribution of hearing loss to the incidence of dementia was greater than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity and social isolation.20 This result seemed surprising, because sensory systems had never been considered particularly important to dementia, except perhaps for olfactory function as a potential biomarker.21 It is important to note that the Livingstone et al study20 was based on data from high-income countries and the evidence from low-to-middle income countries is less convincing in this respect.22 Over the last several years, further evidence in support of the importance of hearing loss for the development of dementia has been published, although it is not entirely consistent.22–27

Less attention has been given to the other part of the inner ear, the vestibular system (see Figure 1); however, evidence is mounting that age-related vestibular disorders could also be a significant risk factor for the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia, along with hearing loss.28 The vestibular system is known to degenerate with age, as with other sensory systems, with decreases in hair cells in the semi-circular canals and otoliths, a reduction in the number of neurons in the vestibular nerve and brainstem vestibular nucleus, and a deterioration of vestibular reflex responses.1,2 As shown in the Iceberg model in Figure 3, clinical presentation of age-related vestibular symptoms usually includes ‘presbystasis’ (the imbalance of disequilibrium) or ‘presbyvertigo’ (vertigo), or both, possibly with a decrease in vestibular perception; however, it is possible for age-related vestibular symptoms to be sub-clinical and therefore harder to detect .

Figure 3: Iceberg model of age-related vestibular loss.

VSR: vestibulo-spinal reflex; VOR: vestibulo-ocular reflex. ‘Presbystasis’: age-related imbalance of disequilibrium. ‘Presbyvertigo’: age-related vertigo. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Animal studies supporting the role of the vestibular system in cognitive function

The literature relating the vestibular system to cognitive function, especially spatial memory, dates back to the 1960’s.14 Numerous studies have reported evidence that unilateral or bilateral lesions of the peripheral vestibular system impair spatial memory in various maze and foraging tasks.14,29–31 In some cases, these have been conducted even 14 months after bilateral vestibular lesions (BVL) in rats, and the spatial memory deficits remain.32 A variety of potentially confounding factors have been controlled for, including vision, degree of motor activity, anxiety and auditory function, and the results have been consistent.29–32 BVL has been demonstrated to impair the function of neurons in the hippocampus that encode places in the environment (‘hippocampal place cells’),11,12 EEG activity in the theta frequency range,33–35 which is thought to regulate place cell function, and theta EEG activity among grid cells of the entorhinal cortex.13 Neurons in the thalamus which encode head direction (‘head direction cells’), are also dysfunctional following BVL.36 Together, these abnormalities in the function of place cells, grid cells and head direction cells, are likely to underlie the spatial memory deficits observed in animals.14,15 Furthermore, transgenic mice without otoconia and therefore without otolith function (‘otolith deficient tilted mice’), but with normal semi-circular canal function, have been shown to have aberrant hippocampal place cell and head direction cell activity.36,37 In addition, a variety of neurochemical changes have been documented in the hippocampus following BVL, including changes in the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype of glutamate receptor and muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptors,38–40 both of which are implicated in hippocampal learning and memory processes.

Taken together, the data from animal studies strongly indicate an important role for the vestibular system in spatial cognition. Although the specific pathways through which vestibular information reaches areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, are yet to be fully elucidated, projections from the vestibular nucleus and cerebellum are likely to transmit information via multiple routes, particularly the thalamus (see Figure 4).41 It is speculated that there is a ‘theta pathway’, which involves projections from the brainstem vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) to the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg), then via various nuclei to the medial septum, which releases ACh into the hippocampus; a ‘head direction pathway’, from the VNC via head direction cells of the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (ADN) to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), to the hippocampus; a major thalamic pathway which transmits vestibular information to the parietal cortex and then on to the MEC and hippocampus; and a transcerebellar pathway.41–43 The detailed pathways are depicted in Figure 4.

Figure 4: ADN, anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; Interpositus N, anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei; LMN, lateral mammillary nuclei; MEC, medial entorhinal cortex; MG, medial geniculate nucleus; NPH, nucleus prepositus hypoglossi; Parietal C, Parietal cortex; PaS, parasubiculum; Perirhinal, Perirhinal cortex; PoS, posterior subiculum (i.e dorsal part of the presubiculum); Post HT, posterior hypothalamus; Postrhinal, postrhinal cortex; PPTg , pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Pulv, pulvinar; RPO, reticularis pontis oralis; SUM, supramammillary nucleus; ViM, ventralis intermedius nuclei of the thalamus; VLN, ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VNC, vestibular nucleus complex; VPi, ventral posterior inferior nucleus of the thalamus; VPL, ventral posterior lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPM, ventral posterior medial nuclei of the thalamus. From Hitier et al.41 with permission.

Vestibular contributions to cognitive function in humans

Consistent with the studies in animals, many studies conducted over the last two decades have demonstrated that vestibular disorders are associated with the impairment of cognitive function in otherwise normal healthy adults.44–53 The studies reviewed were identified using a Pubmed search between 1989 and 2020, using ‘vestibular’ and ‘cognition’ as key words. All of the studies were included; they consisted of two main types: survey and epidemiological studies; and clinical experimental studies (Table 2). Among the symptoms that have been reported are difficulty concentrating, deficits in attention and spatial memory, verbal fluency, mental rotation, and dyscalculia and other forms of numerical cognition (see Table 2). Although some 51 such studies have been published since 1989, only a subset of them (21) have controlled for hearing loss, either by excluding patients with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in the analysis of the data.54–69 Table 2 provides a summary of the studies that have controlled for hearing loss and therefore where the deficits can be considered to be mainly vestibular in origin. Dobbels et al70 have argued that few of the studies reporting cognitive deficits in humans with vestibular disorders, have controlled for hearing loss. However, a careful review of the literature suggests that is not the case. Of course, there are cases in which vestibular and auditory symptoms present together, for example, in Meniere’s disease, in which case they are both likely to contribute to cognitive deficits. Beyond controlling for hearing loss, the major weaknesses of the epidemiological studies are that they often include ‘heterogeneous vestibular disorders’ (bilateral vestibular loss, unilateral vestibular loss, vestibular neuritis, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), etc.) and do not include the same controls as the experimental studies (see Table 2). The available experimental studies are based on samples of patients with different vestibular disorders, but each one of them includes a sample of patients which is compared to a control group without vestibular dysfunction (see Table 2).

Table 2: Studies conducted in humans that have reported cognitive deficits associated with different types of vestibular dysfunction, in which hearing loss has been controlled for in some way, either by excluding subjects with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically in a multiple logistic regression model. Where ‘No ()’ occurs, the first number in the brackets indicates the number of subjects without hearing loss and the second, the total sample size.

Abbreviations: UVL, unilateral vestibular loss; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; UVN, unilateral vestibular neurectomy; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; RT, reaction time.

Studies in humans which have combined structural and functional neuro-imaging with behavioural assays, have also provided compelling evidence that vestibular sensory inputs are important for human spatial cognition. In a seminal study of patients with neurofibromatosis type 2 (NF2) who underwent bilateral vestibular nerve section, it was observed that the NF2 patients exhibited significantly poorer spatial navigation skills, measured using a virtual Morris Water Maze Task, which required no movement other than that of a mouse to control a cursor on a computer screen. These spatial cognitive deficits were correlated with reduced hippocampal volumes (approximately 17%) compared to age- and sex-matched controls.51 Only one of these patients exhibited total hearing loss post-operatively and all of them were 8–10 years post-BVL. Subsequent studies have provided further evidence of impaired spatial memory and hippocampal atrophy in patients with other vestibular disorders such as Meniere’s disease.52,71–74 A recent study of over 100 healthy adults reported that poorer vestibular function was correlated with significantly reduced hippocampal volume.75 In our most recent study, we have found that age is statistically related to a bilateral decrease in the volume of the hippocampus and the left entorhinal cortex.76

Vestibular dysfunction as a risk factor for dementia

As a result of the animal and human studies demonstrating that peripheral vestibular lesions caused spatial memory deficits, Previc77 suggested that loss of vestibular function might be implicated in the development of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). His argument was based partly on the limitations of the β-amyloid (Aβ) hypothesis of AD, but the idea can be considered independently of that hypothesis. The central vestibular system, including the brainstem VNC, contributes to major cholinergic inputs to the hippocampus, which is damaged in AD.78 Bilateral vestibular loss in rats also results in a decrease in acetylcholine (ACh) receptors in the hippocampus.38

There have been only a few studies that have directly investigated the relationship between vestibular function and AD, all of them conducted by the same group. The first studies investigated the statistical relationship between vestibular dysfunction and clinical syndromes of cognitive impairment, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. Harun et al79 investigated the prevalence of vestibular dysfunction in 32 patients with AD and 15 with MCI, compared to 94 controls, and estimated that patients with bilaterally absent cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMPs, a measure of saccular function), had a greater than three-fold increased odds of AD (OR 3.42, 95% CI 1.32–8.91, P=0.011). Furthermore, a 1 µV increase in cVEMP amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.28, 95% CI 0.09–0.93, P=0.038). Higher ocular VEMP (oVEMP, indicative of utricular function) amplitude was associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.92, 95% CI 0.85–0.99, P=0.036). However, there was no significant difference between the MCI group and the controls. Importantly, there was no significant association between VOR function and AD, indicating that semi-circular canal function was not implicated, only the otoliths, the most primitive part of the vestibular system.

In a follow-up study, Wei et al80 examined vestibular function in 51 patients with AD, 26 with MCI and 295 matched controls. The cVEMP, the oVEMP and the VOR were all tested. Compared to controls, they found that people with cVEMP impairment had a 3–4 fold increase in the odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.0, 95% CI 1.1–8.5, P=0.04) and those with oVEMP impairment had an almost four-fold increased odds of being in the MCI group (OR 3.9, 95% CI 1.4–11.3, P=0.01). Compared to controls, they found that people with impaired cVEMPs had a five-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 5.0, 95% CI 2.0–12.3, P=0.001) and those with abnormal oVEMPs had a greater than four-fold increased odds of being in the AD group (OR 4.2, 95% CI 1.9–9.1, P≤0.001). Importantly, VOR gain (the ratio of head velocity to eye velocity) was not significantly related to group membership.

There are a number of limitations of these studies which must be noted, however. First, hearing loss was not controlled for in these studies of AD and MCI, so it is possible that it may have been a contributing factor. Second, the sample sizes of AD patients were relatively small, n=32 and 51, and therefore the studies need to be replicated.79,80 Third, the studies were cross-sectional, and the samples may not have been representative of a broader population.80 In particular, patients with both vestibular and cognitive impairment may have been more likely to present than those with either condition alone, resulting in a potential overestimation of the proportion of vestibular dysfunction in AD (‘Berksonian bias’).80 Fourth, it is conceivable that the poor performance by AD patients on the cVEMP and oVEMP testing could have been due to their inability to understand and follow instructions; however, the authors reported that this was not the case.79,80 Finally, the relationship between cVEMP/oVEMP function and AD was a statistical one involving logistic regression and does not necessarily indicate a causal relationship.79,80 For example, aside from the possibility that vestibular dysfunction contributed to the development of AD, it is possible that AD pathology might have caused vestibular dysfunction.

One potential explanation for the relationship between vestibular dysfunction and AD might be that AD pathology (eg, β-amyloid (Aβ)) extends into the central vestibular pathways from the vestibular nucleus to the thalamus and beyond, thereby impairing vestibular function. Although there have been no specific studies of Aβ deposition in ‘vestibular-related areas’ of the brain, vestibular information is distributed widely,41 (see Figure 4), therefore it is likely that AD pathology extends to many brain regions receiving vestibular input. However, in a recent study of vestibular function in 98 participants aged 77.3 (±8.26) from the BLSA, Aβ deposition was measured using amyloid C-11 Pittsburgh Compound B (11C-PB).81 The authors found that 22.4% of the sample were positive for PiB; however, there was no statistically significant relationship between the extent of Aβ deposition and any measure of vestibular function. This study was designed to investigate preclinical AD, but no such study has been performed in patients diagnosed with AD. Another possible explanation is that vestibular impairment directly contributes to medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration and AD, possibly as a result of reduced vestibular sensory input to areas of the brain such as the hippocampus, as occurs for auditory input.82

Further studies have concentrated on whether vestibular loss is associated with specific phenotypes of AD, especially those with spatial cognitive deficits. Some AD phenotypes are characterised by predominantly amnestic symptoms compared to others which are characterised by more motoric and spatial impairment.83 In a study of 50 patients with MCI or AD, Wei et al84 observed that patients with vestibular loss were significantly more likely to exhibit impairment in neurocognitive tests of spatial skills, for example the Money Road Map test (MRMT). When patients were divided into ‘spatially normal’ and ‘spatially impaired’ groups based on their performance in the MRMT, only 25% of the spatially normal patients were found to have vestibular dysfunction compared to 96% of the spatially impaired patients.84 In a further study of 60 patients with MCI or AD, patients with vestibular dysfunction were significantly more likely to have difficulty driving, an activity closely linked to spatial cognitive ability.85 It is possible, therefore, that vestibular dysfunction contributes to the development of a ‘spatial’ subtype of AD, increasing the probability of symptoms such as spatial disorientation, wandering, and an increased risk of falling.84 However, these two studies have similar limitations to the original one by Harun et al:79 there were no controls for hearing loss, the sample sizes were relatively small, the studies were cross-sectional and in Wei et al,85 the postural measurements were not specific to vestibular function. Finally, the statistical association reported does not indicate causality.

The only available study that provides any evidence that vestibular loss might be causally involved in the development of cognitive impairment and dementia is by Liao et al86 They investigated prior medical conditions that were associated with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia (LOAD) using a population-based matched case control study based on the National Health Insurance Research database of Taiwan and the Catastrophic Illness Certificate database, between the years 1997 and 2013. The definitions of prior diseases were based on the first three digits of the International Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM). The total case group consisted of 4,600 patients who were newly diagnosed with LOAD between 2007 and 2013, who were then matched to 4,600 controls by both age and sex. Using multivariate logistic regression and path analysis, the authors reported that the incidence of LOAD was positively correlated with prior anxiety (ICD code 300), functional digestive disorder (ICD code 564), psychopathology-specific symptoms (ICD code 307), disorders of the vestibular system (ICD code 386), concussion (ICD code 850), disorders of the urethra and urinary tract (ICD code 599), disorders of refraction and accommodation (ICD code 367) and hearing loss (ICD code 389). While the authors conclude that these data suggest that vestibular dysfunction may therefore be a risk factor for LOAD, the limitations of the study include limited information regarding other confounding factors such as body mass index, blood pressure, diet, smoking, diabetic therapy etc.; and the specific nature of the diagnosis may have varied according to factors affecting access to a neurologist. At present, there are no comparable data available on potential vestibular contributions to dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease or fronto-temporal dementia.

One of the intriguing aspects of the studies in cognitively-normal and vestibular-impaired adults is the demonstration of a link between saccular function and cognition.45,47,69,75,76,79,80,85 We have recently reported that saccular function is a statistically significant predictor of the decrease in hippocampal volume that occurs with age.76 The saccule, one of the two otoliths, is the part of the peripheral vestibular system which detects the orientation of the head with respect to gravity and, together with the utricle, is the oldest component of the vestibular system in evolutionary terms (see Figure 2). Patients with AD exhibited specifically poorer saccular and utricular function compared to age-matched controls.79,85 Saccular stimulation has been demonstrated to activate the multisensory vestibular cortex involved in spatial information processing.87,88 In guinea pigs and rats, selective electrical stimulation of the utricle and saccule has been shown to cause widespread activation of the hippocampus.89,90 There is increasing evidence that the otoliths, the saccule in particular, have a critical role in spatial memory due to their importance in the perception of gravitational vertical.19 We have recently reported that mice lacking otolithic function from birth, exhibit major developmental delays by post-natal day 9, including spatial memory deficits.91 Interestingly, considerable electrophysiological evidence suggests that the neurons in the VNC that subserve the VOR are separate from those involved in the VSR pathways and the pathways to the limbic system and neocortex.92 This means that it is possible for the vestibular pathways that give rise to the conscious perception of self-movement and contribute to spatial memory, to be compromised, without VOR deficits necessarily being exhibited, and that only VSRs such as VEMPs would indicate a vestibular deficit. Figure 5 summarises the hypothesis that saccular dysfunction, in particular, might contribute to the development of cognitive dysfunction that preferentially includes spatial cognitive deficits.

Figure 5: Conceptual model of impact of aging on vestibular function (notably saccular function), which contributes to neurodegeneration of neural circuits involved in vestibular processing and deterioration, specifically in spatial cognitive ability. From Agrawal et al28 with permission.

Conclusions

The available evidence suggests that vestibular dysfunction, including that associated with age-related vestibular loss, has a significant negative impact on cognitive function. Vestibular impairment may therefore be a risk factor for the development of dementia, including AD. Previc93 has recently suggested that, given the increased prevalence of vestibular disorders in females,94 vestibular dysfunction may contribute to their increased incidence of AD. However, the majority of the evidence to date is correlational; therefore, caution must be exercised in interpreting these findings. The potential combined effects of both hearing loss and vestibular loss are unknown but could be expected to be much greater. Of course, it is important to note that some otological disorders involve both auditory and vestibular symptoms (eg, Meniere’s disease). Understanding the full implications of vestibular dysfunction for cognitive decline is potentially of great importance for the health of the elderly, since effective therapies are available to treat vestibular disorders.95 One of the principal treatments for vestibular impairment is vestibular rehabilitation, a suite of physical therapy-based exercises in which head movements are used to stimulate the vestibular system and gradually encourage the brain to adapt to the loss of normal vestibular function.96 Several studies have reported that vestibular rehabilitation can improve cognitive function in healthy adults and in patients with intractable dizziness.67,97 However, at present, studies from the US indicate that only a small number of people with AD are referred for vestibular rehabilitation.98 Since vestibular impairment may be a modifiable risk factor for dementia, the impact of vestibular loss on cognition should be considered along with hearing loss as a critical area for research.

Summary

Abstract

Dementia is recognised to be one of the most challenging diseases facing society, both now and in the future, with its prevalence estimated to increase substantially by 2050. The potential contributions of age-related sensory deficits have attracted little attention until recently, when a landmark study suggested that hearing loss could be a greater risk factor for dementia than hypertension, obesity, smoking, depression, physical inactivity or social isolation. Over the last decade, evidence has been gradually accumulating to suggest that the other part of the inner ear, the balance organs or ‘vestibular system’, might also be important in the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Increasing evidence suggests that dizziness associated with vestibular dysfunction, a common reason for patients consulting their GPs, increases the risk of cognitive dysfunction, including dementia, and our understanding of the basic neurobiology of this sensory system supports this view. This paper aims to review and critically evaluate the relevant evidence.

Aim

Method

Results

Conclusion

Author Information

Paul F Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the three anonymous referees for their constructive and helpful comments on the manuscript.

Correspondence

Prof Paul Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.

Correspondence Email

paul.smith@otago.ac.nz

Competing Interests

Nil.

1. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Merfeld D. Dizziness, imbalance and age-related vestibular loss. In: Straka H, editor, Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Elsevier, NY, 2020.

2. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, et al. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the national health and nutrition examination survey, 2001-2004. Arch. Intern. Med. 2009; 169(10):938–944.

3. Health Quality and Safety Commission New Zealand, 2020. http://public.tableau.com/profile/hqi2803#!/vizhome/FallsFracturesOutcomesFramework/Landing

4. Hartholt KA, Lee R, Burns ER, Van Beeck EF. Mortality from falls among US adults aged 75 years or older, 2000–2016. JAMA. 2019; 321(21):2131–2133.

5. Agrawal Y, Merfeld D, Horak F, et al. Aging, vestibular function and balance: Proceedings of a National Institute on Aging/National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders Workshop. J. Gerontol.: Med. Sci. 2020, in press.

6. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ, Angelaki DE, et al. The Vestibular System: A Sixth Sense. New York: Oxford University Press, 2012.

7. Curthoys IS, Grant JW, Burgess AM, et al. Otolith receptor mechanisms for vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials: A review. Front. Neurol. 2018; 9:366.

8. Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang Y, et al. Anatomy and surgical approach to rat’s vestibular sensors and nerves. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2016; 270:1–8.

9. Urciuoli A, Zanolli C, Beaudet A, et al. The evolution of the vestibular apparatus in apes and humans. Elife. 2020; 9. pii:e51261.

10. Ramos de Miguel A, Zarowski A, Sluydts M, et al. The superiority of the otolith system.  Audiol Neurootol. 2020; 25(1–2):35–41.

11. Stackman RW, Clark AS, Taube JS. Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input. Hippocampus 2002; 12:291–303.

12. Russell NA, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Long-term effects of permanent vestibular lesions on hippocampal spatial firing. J. Neurosci. 2003; 23(16):6490–6498.

13. Jacob P-Y, Poucet B, Liberge M, et al. Vestibular control of entorhinal cortex activity in spatial navigation. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:38.

14. Besnard S, Lopez C, Brandt T, et al. (editors) The vestibular system in cognitive and memory processes in mammals. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. Lausanne: Frontiers Media. doi: 10.3389/978-2-88919-744-6 (ebook) pp. 1–246, 2016.

15. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2017; 30(1):84–89.

16. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971; 34(1):171–5.

17. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, et al. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2015; 436(7052):801–6.

18. Moser EI, Moser MB, McNaughton BL. Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1448–1464.

19. Smith PF. The growing evidence for the importance of the otoliths for spatial memory. Front. Neural Circ. 2019; 3(66):1–14.

20. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017; 390(10113):2673–2734.

21. Bathini P, Brai E, Auber LA. Olfactory dysfunction in the pathophysiological continuum of dementia. Ageing Res Rev. 2019; 55:100956.

22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntly J, et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. The Lancet Global Health. 2019; 7:e596–e603.

23. Michalowsky B, Hoffmann W, Kostev K. Association between hearing and vision impairment and risk of dementia: Results of a case-control study based on secondary data. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 20;11: 363.

24. Golub JS, Brickman AM, Ciarleglio AJ, et al. Association of subclinical hearing loss with cognitive performance. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2020; 146(1):57–67.

25. Loughrey DG, Parra MA, Lawlor BA. Visual short-term memory binding deficit with age-related hearing loss in cognitively normal older adults. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1):12600.

26. Liu CM, Lee CT. Association of hearing loss with dementia. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(7): e198112.

27. Curhan SG, Willett WC, Grodstein F, et al. Longitudinal study of self-reported hearing loss and subjective cognitive function decline in women. Alzheimers Dement. 2020; 16(4):610–620.

28. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Rosenberg PB. Vestibular impairment, cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s Disease: Balancing the evidence. Aging and Mental Health. 2020; 24(5):705–708.

29. Smith PF, Zheng Y. From ear to uncertainty: Vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2013; 7:84.

30. Smith PF, Geddes LH, Baek J-H, et al. Modulation of cognitive function by vestibular lesions and galvanic vestibular stimulation. Front. Neurol. 2010; 1(141):1–8.

31. Smith PF, Darlington CL, Zheng Y. The effects of complete vestibular deafferentation on spatial memory and the hippocampus in rat: The Dunedin experience. In: Ferre, E.R, Harris, L. (editors), Vestibular Cognition. Brill, Leiden, 2017; pp. 461–485.

32. Baek JH, Zheng Y, Darlington C L, et al. Evidence that spatial memory deficits following bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats are probably permanent. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2010; 94(3):402–413.

33. Russell N, Horii A, Smith PF, et al. Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2006; 96:4–14.

34. Neo P, Carter, D, Zheng, Y, et al. Septal elicitation of hippocampal theta rhythm did not repair the cognitive and emotional deficits resulting from vestibular lesions. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:1176–1187.

35. Tai SK, Ma J, Ossenkopp KP, et al. Activation of immobility-related hippocampal theta by cholinergic septohippocampal neurons during vestibular stimulation. Hippocampus. 2012; 22(4):914–25.

36. Cullen KE, Taube JS. Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges.  Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20(11):1465–1473.

37. Harvey RE, Rutan SA, Willey GR, et al. M. Linear self-motion cues support the spatial distribution and stability of hippocampal place cells. Curr. Biol. 2018; 28(11):1810.e5.

38. Aitken P, Benoit A, Zheng Y, et al. Hippocampal and striatal M1-muscarinic acetylcholine receptors are down-regulated following bilateral vestibular loss in rats. Hippocampus. 2016; 26:1509–1514.

39. Besnard S, Machado ML, Vignaux G, et al. Influence of vestibular input on spatial and nonspatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors. Hippocampus. 2012; 22:814–826.

40. Truchet B, Benoit A, Chaillan F, et al. Hippocampal LTP modulation and glutamatergic receptors following vestibular loss. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019; 224:699–711.

41. Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 2014; 8:59.

42. Bohne P, Schwarz MK, Herlitze S, Mark MD. A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Front Neural Circuits. 2019; 13:51.

43. Watson TC, Obiang P, Torres-Herraez A, et al. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife. 2019; 8:e41896.

44. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Du Lac S, et al. Vestibular vertigo and comorbid cognitive and psychiatric impairment: The 2008 national health interview survey. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. 2016; 87(4):310–319.

45. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Trevino C, et al. Association between visuo- spatial ability and vestibular function in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 2015; 63(9):1837–1844.

46. Semenov YR, Bigelow RT, Xue Q, et al. Association between vestibular and cognitive function in US adults: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J. Gerontol. Series A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015; 71(2):243–250.

47. Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, et al. Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: Data from the Triangle Completion Task. Front Neurol. 2017; 8:173.

48. Wackym PA, Balaban CD, Mackay HT, et al. Longitudinal cognitive and neurobehavioral functional outcomes before and after repairing otic capsule dehiscence. Otol Neurotol. 2016; 37(1):70–82.

49. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol. 2017; 264(3):554–563.

50. Smith L, Wilkinson D, Bodani M, et al. Short-term memory impairment in vestibular patients can arise independently of psychiatric impairment, fatigue, and sleeplessness. J Neuropsychol. 2019; 13(3):417–431.

51. Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. 2005; 128(11):2732–2741.

52. Kremmyda O, Huffner K, Flanagin VL, et al. Beyond dizziness: Virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front. Human Neurosci. 2016; 10:139.

53. Ayar DA, Kumral E, Celebisoy N. Cognitive functions in acute vestibular loss. J. Neurol. 2020, in press. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w.

54. Sang FY, Jáuregui-Renaud K, Green DA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms in vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006; 77(6):760–6.

55. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang FY, Gresty MA, et al. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008; 79(3):276–83.

56. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolaños M, et al. Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res. 2008; 18(2–3):129–37.

57. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, Hoffman HJ, et al. Association between vertigo, cognitive and psychiatric conditions in US children: 2012 National Health Interview Survey. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2020; 130:109802.

58. Risey J, Briner W. Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J. Vest. Res. 1990; 1:31–37.

59. Redfern MS, Talkowski ME, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive influences on postural control of patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Gait and Posture. 2004; 19:105–114.

60. Talkowski ME, Redfern MS, Jennings JR, et al. Cognitive requirements for vestibular and ocular motor processing in healthy adults and patients with unilateral vestibular lesions. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2005; 17(9):1432–1441.

61. Gomez-Alvarez FB, Jáuregui-Renaud K. Psychological symptoms and spatial orientation during the first 3 months after acute vestibular neuritis. Arch. Med. Res. 2011; 42:97–103.

62. Caixeta GC dos S, Dona F, Gazzola JM. Cognitive processing and body balance in elderly subjects with vestibular dysfunction. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2012; 78(2):87–95.

63. Candidi M, Micarelli A, Viziano A, et al. Impaired mental rotation in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and acute vestibular neuritis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013; 7:783.

64. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Acute peripheral vestibular deficit increases the redundancy in random number generation. Exp. Brain Res. 2017; 235:627–637.

65. Moser I, Vibert D, Caversaccio MD, et al. Impaired math achievement in patients with acute vestibular neuritis. Neuropsychologia. 2017; 107:1–8.

66. Lofti Y, Rezazadeh N, Moossavi A, et al. Preliminary evidence of improved cognitive performance following vestibular rehabilitation in children with combined ADHD (cADHD) and concurrent vestibular impairment. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2017; 44:700–707.

67. Sugaya N, Arai M, Goto F. Changes in cognitive function in patients with intractable dizziness following vestibular rehabilitation. Sci. Reps. 2018; 8:9984.

68. Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, et al. Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs embodied spatial cognition. J. Neurol. 2019; 266(Suppl. 1: S149–S159.

69. Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei et al. Association between saccule and semi-circular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear. 2020; 41:686–692.

70. Dobbels B, Peetermans O, Boon B, et al. Impact of bilateral vestibulopathy on spatial and nonspatial cognition: A systematic review. Ear Hear. 2018; 40(4):757–765.

71. zu Eulenburg P, Stoeter P, Dieterich M. Voxel-based morphometry depicts central compensation after vestibular neuritis. Annal. Neurol. 2010; 68:241–249.

72. Van Cruijsen N, Hiemstra WM, Meiners LC, et al. Hippocampal volume measurement in patients with Ménière’s disease: a pilot study. Acta Otolaryngol. 2007; 127(10):1018–23.

73. Seo YJ, Kim J, Kim SH. The change of hippocampal volume and its relevance with inner ear function in Meniere’s disease patients. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2016; 43(6):620–5.

74. Göttlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, et al. Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016; 37(5):1998–2006.

75. Kamil RJ, Jacob A, Ratnanather JT, et al. Vestibular function and hippocampal volume in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA). Otol. Neurotol. 2018; (6):765–771.

76. Jacob A, Tward D, Resnick S, et al. Vestibular function and cortical and sub-cortical alterations in an aging population. Heliyon. 2020, 6: e04728.

77. Previc FH. Vestibular loss as a contributor to Alzheimer’s disease. Med. Hypoth. 2013; 80(4):360–367.

78. Chapleau M, Aldebert J, Montembeault M, Brambati SM. Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease and Semantic Dementia: An ALE Meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016; 54(3):941–955.

79. Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow RT, et al. Vestibular impairment in dementia. Otol. Neurotol. 2016; 37(8):1137–1142.

80. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Vestibular loss predicts poorer spatial cognition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017; 61(3):995–1003.

81. Kamil RJ, Bilgel M, Wong DF, et al. Vestibular function and beta-amyloid deposition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.  Front Aging Neurosci. 2018; 10:408.

82. Cui B, Zhu L, She X, et al. Chronic noise exposure causes persistence of tau hyperphosphorylation and formation of NFT tau in the rat hippocampus and pre- frontal cortex. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 238(2):122–129.

83. Mapstone M, Steffenella TM, Duffy CJ. A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment: getting lost between aging and AD. Neurol. 2003; 60(5):802–808.

84. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Increased prevalence of vestibular loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2019; 16(12):1143–1150.

85. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et al. Saccular impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with driving difficulty. Dement. Geriatric Cog. Dis. 2017; 44(5–6):294–302.

86. Liao JY, Lee CT, Lin TY, et al. Exploring prior diseases associated with incident late-onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia. PLoS One. 2020; 15(1):e0228172.

87. Miyamoto T, Fukushima K, Takada T, et al. Saccular projections in the human cerebral cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2005; 1039(1):124–131.

88. Schlindwein P, Mueller M, Bauermann T, et al. Cortical representation of saccular vestibular stimulation: VEMPs in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2008; 39(1):19–31.

89. Cuthbert PC, Gilchrist DP, Hicks SL, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for vestibular activation of the guinea pig hippocampus. Neuroreport. 2000; 11(7):1443–47.

90. Smith PF, Hitier M, Zhang, Y-F, et al. Vestibular modulation of hippocampal and striatal function. J. Vest. Res. 2019; 29(1):18.

91. Le Gall A, Hilber P, Chesneau C, et al. The critical role of vestibular graviception during cognitive-motor development. Behav. Brain Res. 2019; 372:112040.

92. Cullen KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control.  Trends Neurosci. 2012; 35(3):185–96.

93. Previc F. Does vestibular dysfunction contribute to the increased prevalence of Alzheimer’s Disease in females? Clin. Case. Revs. 2019; 2(1):1–2.

94. Smith PF, Agrawal Y, Darlington CL. Sexual dimorphism in vestibular function and dysfunction. J. Neurophysiol. 2019; 121:2379–2391.

95. Hillier S, McDonnell M. Is vestibular rehabilitation effective in improving dizziness and function after unilateral peripheral vestibular hypofunction? an abridged version of a Cochrane Review. Eur. J. Phys. Rehab. Med. 2016; 52(4):541–556.

96. Herdman SJ, Clendaniel R. Vestibular Rehabilitation (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis. 2014.

97. Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech A, et al. Balance training improves memory and spatial cognition in healthy adults.  Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1):5661.

98. Gandhi P, Klatt BN, Agrawal Y. Physical and vestibular physical therapy referrals in people with Alzheimer Disease.  Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000390.

Contact diana@nzma.org.nz
for the PDF of this article

Subscriber Content

The full contents of this pages only available to subscribers.
Login, subscribe or email nzmj@nzma.org.nz to purchase this article.

LOGINSUBSCRIBE
No items found.